Dr. Terry Sejnowski: How to Improve at Learning Using Neuroscience & AI
en
November 18, 2024
TLDR: Discussion with Dr. Terry Sejnowski on effective learning strategies, AI tools for knowledge acquisition, non-AI strategies like exercise for cognitive enhancement, and how computational methods and AI are impacting personal health and treating brain diseases such as Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s.
Dr. Terry Sejnowski, a leading computational neuroscientist from the Salk Institute, provides an intriguing perspective on enhancing learning through neuroscience and the application of artificial intelligence (AI). In this episode of the podcast, various innovative methods are discussed, emphasizing both biological foundations and practical applications for improving cognitive abilities. Below are key topics and insights derived from the conversation.
Understanding the Brain’s Learning Mechanisms
- Computational Neuroscience: Sejnowski emphasizes the importance of understanding how neurons communicate and process information. He uses mathematical models to simplify complex brain functions, making the neuroscience field more accessible to a wider audience.
- Motivation and Dopamine: Motivation is driven by dopamine, a critical neurotransmitter that governs learning and reward mechanisms. Understanding how dopamine functions can help individuals master new skills more effectively.
Effective Learning Strategies
- AI Tools for Learning: Sejnowski presents AI as a powerful tool to aid in the acquisition of knowledge. AI can help in foraging information, generating ideas, and assisting in analyzing complex data, particularly in healthcare settings.
- Active Learning: The discussion shifts towards active learning strategies rather than passive memorization. This includes the significance of procedural learning—mastering skills through practice, which is often more effective than theoretical learning alone.
- Example: Learning to play tennis requires practice and repetition to build muscle memory, just as acquiring medical knowledge needs continual application to transmit theoretical learning into practical knowledge.
The Role of Exercise in Cognitive Function
- Mitochondrial Health: Dr. Sejnowski outlines the link between physical exercise, mitochondrial health, and cognitive performance. Specific types of exercise can enhance brain function and creativity, underscoring the importance of regular physical activity for cognitive vitality.
- Cognitive Velocity: The concept of cognitive velocity is discussed, which refers to the ability to process information swiftly and efficiently. Engaging in both cognitive and physical exercises enhances one’s overall cognitive control.
Impact on Mental Health and Neurological Diseases
- Addressing Neurodegenerative Conditions: The episode explores how advancements in computational neuroscience and AI could facilitate innovations in treating conditions such as Alzheimer's and Parkinson’s disease. For instance, enhancing mitochondrial function might provide a pathway to improving symptoms or slowing disease progression.
- Importance of Sleep: Sejnowski highlights the role of sleep in consolidating knowledge. The need for sufficient sleep to experience beneficial memory spindles during non-REM sleep is emphasized.
The Integration of AI in Scientific Research
- Transformative Power of AI: The discussion involves how AI can be employed as an idea pump in research. By analyzing existing literature and generating hypotheses, researchers can drive innovation forward.
- AI in Predictions: The potential for AI to predict future outcomes based on vast datasets is elucidated. Sejnowski notes that AI's ability to assimilate information from diverse sources could revolutionize fields such as healthcare and education.
Final Thoughts
Dr. Sejnowski’s insights serve as a gateway for individuals looking to enhance their learning experiences through both biological understanding and innovative technologies. As we delve deeper into the worlds of neuroscience and AI, leveraging these insights could lead to not only personal growth but also significant advancements in educational methods and mental health treatment.
In summary, this podcast episode highlights:
- The fundamental principles of how the brain learns and remembers.
- The synergistic roles of neuroscience and AI in improving cognitive abilities and mental health.
- Practical applications of exercise and active learning strategies for personal development.
For anyone interested in understanding the mechanisms of learning and executing effective educational strategies, this conversation with Dr. Terry Sejnowski offers invaluable knowledge.
Was this summary helpful?
Welcome to the Huberman Lab Podcast where we discuss science and science-based tools for everyday life.
I'm Andrew Huberman, and I'm a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology at Stanford School of Medicine. My guest today is Dr. Terry Sainowski. Dr. Terry Sainowski is a professor at the Salk Institute for Biological Studies, where he directs the Computational Neurobiology Laboratory. And as his title suggests, he is a computational neuroscientist. That is, he uses math as well as artificial intelligence and computing methods to understand this overarching, ultra important question of how the brain works.
Now, I realize that when people hear terms like computational neuroscience, algorithms, large language models, and AI, that it can be a bit overwhelming and even intimidating. But I assure you that the purpose of Dr. Sainowski's work, and indeed today's discussion, is all about using those methods to clarify how the brain works, and indeed to simplify the answer to that question.
So, for instance, today you will learn that regardless of who you are, regardless of your experience, that all your motivation in all domains of life is governed by a simple algorithm or equation. Dr. Signowski explains how a single rule, a single learning rule, drives all of our motivation-related behaviors. And it, of course, relates to the neuromodulator dopamine. And if you're familiar with dopamine as a term, today you will really understand how dopamine works.
to drive your levels of motivation or, in some cases, lack of motivation, and how to overcome that lack of motivation. Today, we also discuss how best to learn. Dr. Sennowski shares not just information about how the brain works, but also practical tools that he and colleagues have developed, including a zero-cost online portal that teaches you how to learn better based on your particular learning style.
the way that you in particular forage for information and implement that information. Dr. Signowski also explains how he himself uses physical exercise of a particular type in order to enhance his cognition, that is his brain's ability to learn information and to come up with new ideas. Today we also discuss both the healthy brain and the diseased brain.
in conditions like Parkinson's and Alzheimer's, and how particular tools that relate to mitochondrial function can perhaps be used in order to treat various diseases including Alzheimer's dementia. I'm certain that by the end of today's episode, you will have learned a tremendous amount of new knowledge about how your brain works and practical tools that you can implement in your daily life.
Before we begin, I'd like to emphasize that this podcast is separate from my teaching and research roles at Stanford. It is, however, part of my desired effort to bring zero cost to consumer information about science and science-related tools to the general public. In keeping with that theme, I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast.
Our first sponsor is BetterHelp. BetterHelp offers professional therapy with a licensed therapist carried out completely online. I've been doing weekly therapy for well over 30 years. Initially, I didn't have a choice. It was a condition of being allowed to stay in school, but pretty soon I realized that therapy is an extremely important component to one's overall health. In fact, I consider doing regular therapy just as important as getting regular exercise, including cardiovascular exercise and resistance training, which of course, I also do every single week.
Now, there are essentially three things that great therapy provides. First of all, it provides a good rapport with somebody that you can trust and talk to about essentially all issues that you want to. Second of all, great therapy provides support in the form of emotional support or simply directed guidance, what to do or what not to do in given areas of your life.
And third, expert therapy can provide you useful insights that you would not have been able to arrive at on your own. BetterHelp makes it very easy to find an expert therapist who you really resonate with and that can provide you the benefits I just mentioned that come with effective therapy. If you'd like to try BetterHelp, go to betterhelp.com slash Huberman to get 10% off your first month. Again, that's betterhelp.com slash Huberman. Today's episode is also brought to us by Helix Sleep.
Helix Sleep makes mattresses and pillows that are customized to your unique sleep needs. Now, I've spoken many times before on this and other podcasts about the fact that getting a great night's sleep is the foundation of mental health, physical health, and performance. Now, the mattress you sleep on makes a huge difference in terms of the quality of sleep that you get each night. How soft it is or how firm it is, how breathable it is, all play into your comfort and need to be tailored to your unique sleep needs.
If you go to the Helix website, you can take a brief two-minute quiz, and it asks you questions such as, do you sleep on your back, your side, or your stomach? Do you tend to run hot or cold during the night? Things of that sort. Maybe you know the answers to those questions, maybe you don't. Either way, Helix will match you to the ideal mattress for you. For me, that turned out to be the dusk mattress, the USK. I started sleeping on a dusk mattress about three and a half years ago, and it's been far and away the best sleep that I've ever had. If you'd like to try Helix, you can go to helixsleep.com slash Huberman.
Take that two minute sleep quiz and Helix will match you to a mattress that is customized for your unique sleep needs. For the month of November 2024, Helix is giving up to 25% off on all mattress orders and two free pillows. Again, that's helixsleep.com slash Huberman to get up to 25% off and two free pillows. And now for my discussion with Dr. Terry Sainowski. Dr. Terry Sainowski. Welcome.
Great to be here. We go way back. And I'm a huge, huge fan of your work because you've worked on a great many different things in the field of neuroscience. You're considered by many a computational neuroscience, so you bring mathematical models to an understanding of the brain and neural networks. And we're also going to talk about AI today. And we're going to make it accessible for everybody, biologist or no, math background or no. To kick things off, I want to understand something.
I understand a bit about the parts list of the brain, and most listeners of this podcast will understand a little bit of the parts list of the brain, even if they've never heard an episode of this podcast before, because they understand their cells. Those cells are neurons. Those neurons connect to one another in very specific ways. They're allowed to see, to hear, to think, et cetera. But I've come to the belief that even if we know the parts list,
It doesn't really inform us how the brain works. This is the big question. How does the brain work? What is consciousness? All of this stuff.
So where and how does an understanding of how neurons talk to one another start to give us a real understanding about like how the brain works? Like what is this piece of meat in our heads? Because it can't just be, okay, the hippocampus remembers stuff and the, you know, the visual cortex perceives stuff. When you sit back and you remove the math from the mental conversation, if that's possible for you,
How do you think about, quote unquote, how the brain works? Like at a very basic level, what is this piece of meat in our heads really trying to accomplish? Let's just say the time when we first wake up in the morning and we're a little groggy till we make it to that first cup of coffee or water. Or maybe even just to urinate first thing in the morning. What is going on in there? What a great question. And you know, I have a,
Pat Church Lane, I wrote a book, Computers Her Brain, and in it, there's this levels diagram.
And it levels of investigation at different spatial scales from the molecular at the very bottom to synapses and neurons, circuits, neural circuits, how they're connected with each other, and then brain areas in the cortex and then the whole central nervous system span 10 orders of magnitude, you know, 10th to the 10th in spatial scale. So, you know, where is consciousness in all of that?
So there are two approaches that neuroscientists have taken. I shouldn't say neuroscientists, I should say that scientists have taken.
And the one you describe, which is, you know, let's look at all the parts, that's the bottom-up approach. You know, take it apart and just see the reductionist approach. And you make a lot of progress. You can figure out, you know, how things are connected and understand how development works, how neurons connect. But it's very difficult to really make progress because quickly you get lost in the forest.
Now, the other approach, which has been successful, but at the end on satisfying is the top-down approach. And this is the approach that psychologists have taken looking at behavior and trying to understand the laws of behavior. This is the behaviorist. But even people in AI were trying to do a top-down to write programs.
that could replicate human behavior, intelligent behavior. I had to say that both of those approaches, bottom up or top down, have really not gotten to the core of answering any of those questions, the big questions. But there's a whole new approach now that is emerging in both neuroscience and AI at exactly the same time. At this moment in history, it's really quite remarkable.
So there's an intermediate level between the implementation level at the bottom, how you implement some particular mechanism. And the actual behavior of the whole system is called the algorithmic.
level. It's in between. So algorithms are like recipes. They're like, you know, when you bake a cake, you have to have ingredients and you have to say the order in which they're put together and how long and, you know, if you get it wrong, you know, it doesn't work. You know, it's just a mess. Now, it turns out that we're discovering algorithms. We've made a lot of progress.
with understanding the algorithms that are used in neural circuits. And this speaks to the computational level of how to understand the function of the neural circuit. But I'm going to give you one example of an algorithm, which is when we worked on, back in the 1990s, when Peter Dayan and Reed Monogue were post-docs in the lab.
And it had to do with the part of the brain below the cortex, called the basal ganglia, which is responsible for learning sequences of actions in order to achieve some goal.
For example, if you want to play tennis, you have to be able to coordinate many muscles and a whole sequence of actions has to be made if you want to be able to serve accurately. And you have to practice, practice, practice. Well, what's going on there is that the basal ganglia basically is taking over from the cortex and producing actions that get better and better and better and better. And that's true not just of the muscles, but it's also true of thinking.
If you want to become good in any area, if you want to become a good financier, if you want to become a good doctor,
or neuroscientists, you have to be practicing, practicing, practicing in terms of understanding what's the details of the profession and what works, what doesn't work, and so forth. And it turns out that this basal ganglia interacts with the cortex, not just in the back, which is the action part, but also with the prefrontal cortex, which is the thinking part. Can I ask you a question about this briefly? The basal ganglia, as I understand, are involved in
the organization of two major types of behaviors go meaning to actually perform a behavior but the basal ganglia also instruct no go don't don't engage in that behavior and learning a an expert golf swing or even a basic golf swing or tennis racket swing involves both of those things go and no go given what you just said which is that the basal ganglia are also involved in
generating thoughts of particular kinds. I wonder, therefore, if it's also involved in suppression of thoughts of particular kinds. You don't want your surgeon cutting into a particular region and just thinking about their motor behaviors, what to do and what not to do. They presumably need to think about
what to think about, but also what to not think about. You don't want that surgeon thinking about how their kid was a brat that morning and they're frustrated because the two things interact. So is there go no go in terms of action and learning and is there go no go in terms of thinking? Well, I mentioned the prefrontal cortex and that part, the loop with the basal ganglia, that is one of the last to mature in early adulthood.
And, you know, what the problem is that for adolescents, it's not the no-go part for, you know, planning and actions. This isn't quite there yet. And so often it doesn't kick in to prevent you from doing things that are not in your best interest. So, yes, absolutely right. But one of the things, though, is that learning is involved. And this is really a problem that we cracked
first theoretically in the 90s and then experimentally later by recording from neurons and also brain imaging in humans. So it turns out we know the algorithm that is used in the brain for how to learn sequences of actions to achieve a goal. And it's the simplest possible algorithm you can imagine. It's simply to predict
The next reward you're going to get, if I do an action, will it give me something of value? And you learn every time you try something, whether you've got the amount of reward you expected or less, you use that to update the synapses.
synaptic plasticity so that the next time you'll have a better chance of getting a better reward. And you build up what's called a value function. And so the cortex now over your lifetime is building up a lot of knowledge about things that are good for you, things that are bad for you. Like you go to a restaurant, you order something, how do you know what's good for you? You've had lots of meals in a lot of places. And now that is part of your value function.
This is the same algorithm that was used by AlphaGo. This is the program that DeepMind built. This is an AI program that beat the world Go champion. And Go is the most complex game that humans have ever
played on a regular basis. Far more complex than chess, as I understand. Yeah, that's right. So, go with the chess with chess is to something like checkers. In other words, the level of difficulty is another way off above it because you have to think in terms of battles going on all over the place at the same time and the order in which you put the pieces down are going to affect what's going to happen in the future.
So this value function is super interesting. And I wonder whether, and I think you answered this, but I wonder whether this value function is implemented over long periods of time. So you talked about the value function in terms of learning a motor skill.
Let's say swinging a tennis racket to do a perfect tennis serve or even just a decent tennis serve. When somebody goes back to the court, let's say on the weekend, once a month over the course of years, are they able to tap into that same value function every time they go back, even though there's been a lot of intervening time and learning? That's question number one. And then the other question is, do you think that this value function is also being played out
in more complex scenarios, not just motor learning, such as, let's say, a domain of life that for many people involves some trial and error, it would be like human relationships. We learn how to be friends with people. We learn how to be a good sibling. We learn how to be a good romantic partner. We get some things right. We get some things wrong. So as the same value function being implemented, we're paying attention to what was rewarding
But what I didn't hear you say also was what was punishing. So are we only paying attention to what is rewarding? Or are we also integrating punishment? We don't get an electric shock when we get the serve wrong, but we can be frustrated.
What you identified is some very important feature, which is that rewards, by the way, every time you do something, you're updating this value function every time. And it accumulates. And the answer to your first question, the answer is that it's always going to be there. It doesn't matter. It's a very permanent part of your experience and who you are.
And interestingly, and the behaviors knew this back in the 1950s, that you can get there two ways of trial and error. Smaller words are good because you're constantly coming closer and closer to getting what you're seeking, better tennis player or being able to make a friend. But the negative,
Punishment is much more effective. One trial learning.
You don't need to have, you know, 100 trials to, you know, what you need, you know, when you're training a rat to do some tasks with small food rewards. But if you just shock the rat, boy, that rat doesn't forget that. Yeah, one really bad relationship will have you learning certain things forever. And this is also PTSD, post-traumatic stress disorder is another good example of that. That can screw you up for the rest of your life.
So, but the other thing, and you pointed out something really important, which is that a large part of the prefrontal cortex is devoted to social interactions. And this is how humans, when you come into the world, you don't know what language you're going to be speaking. You don't know what the cultural values are that you're going to have to be able to become a member of this
society and as things that are expected of you, all of that has to come through experience through building this value function. And this is something we discovered in the 20th century. And now it's going into AI. It's called reinforcement learning in AI. It's a form of procedural learning as opposed to the cognitive level where you think and you do things. Cognitive thinking is much less efficient.
because you have to go step by step with procedural learning. It's automatic. Can you give me an example of procedural learning in the context of a comparison to cognitive learning? Like, is there an example of
perhaps like how to make a decent cup of coffee using purely knowledge-based learning versus procedural learning. Where procedural learning wins. And I can imagine one, but you're the true expert here. Well, you know a lot of examples, but since we've been talking about tennis, can you imagine learning how to play tennis through a book, reading a book? That's so funny. On the plane back from Nashville yesterday, the guy sitting across the aisle from me
was reading a book about maybe just working on his pilot's license or something. And I looked over and couldn't help but notice these diagrams of the plane flying and I thought, I'm just so glad that this guy is a passenger and not a pilot. And then I thought about how the pilots learned and presumably it was a combination of
practical learning and textbook learning. I mean, when you scuba dive, this is true. I'm scuba dive certified. And when you get your certification, you learn your dive tables and you learn why you have to wait between dives, et cetera, and gas exchange and a number of things.
there's really no way to simulate what it is to take your mask off underwater, put it back on, and then blow the water out of your mask. You just have to do that in a pool, and you actually have to do it when you need to, for it to really get drilled in. It's really essential for things that have to be executed quickly and expertly to get that really down pat so you don't have to think.
This happens in school. In other words, you have classroom lessons where you're given explicit instruction, but then you go do homework. That's procedural learning. You do problems. You solve problems.
And I'm a PhD physicist, so I went through all of the classes in theoretical physics. And it was really the problems that really were the core of becoming a good physicist. You can memorize the equations, but that doesn't mean you understand how to use the equations. I think it's worth highlighting something a lot of times on this podcast.
We talk about what I call protocols. It would be, you know, like, get some morning sunlight in your eyes to stimulate your suprachiasmatic nucleus by way of your retinal ganglion cells. Audiences of this podcast will recognize those terms. It's basically get sunlight in your eyes in the morning and set your circadian clock. And you can hear that a trillion times. But I do believe that there's some value to both knowing what the protocol is, the underlying mechanisms. There are these things in your eye that, you know, encode the sunrise,
qualities of light, et cetera, and then send them to your brain, et cetera, et cetera. But then once we link knowledge, pure knowledge, to a practice, I do believe that the two things merge someplace in a way that, let's say reinforces both the knowledge and the practice. So these things are not necessarily separate. They bridge. In other words, doing your theoretical physics problem sets reinforces the examples that you learned in lecture and in your textbooks, and vice versa.
So this is a battle that's going on right now in schools. What you just said is absolutely right. You need both. We have two major learning systems. We have a cognitive learning system, which is critical. We have a procedural learning system, which is sub-critical, basal ganglia.
And the two go hand in hand, if you want to become good at anything, the two are going to help each other. And what's going on right now in schools, in California at least, is that they're trying to get rid of the procedural.
That's ridiculous. They don't want students to practice because it's going to be, you know, you're stressing them. You don't want them to be, to feel that, you know, that they're having difficulty. So, but we can, but it can do everything. I was listening, I'm covering my eyes because, I mean, this would, this would be like saying, um, goodness, there's so many examples. Like here's a textbook on swimming and then you're, you're going to go out to the ocean someday and you will have never actually swum. Right.
And now you're expected to be able to survive, let alone swim well. It's crazy. It's crazy. And I'll tell you, Barbara Oakley has, and I have a MOOC, Massive Open Online Course on learning how to learn. And it helps students, we aimed it at students, but it actually has been taken by 4 million people in 200 countries ages 10 to 90. What is this called? Learning how to learn. Is it, is there a paywall?
No, it's free, completely free. Amazing. And, you know, I get incredible feedback, you know, fan letters almost every day. Well, you're about to get a few more. Okay. I did an episode on learning how to learn and my understanding of the research is that we need to test ourselves on the material. The testing is not just a form of evaluation, it is a form of
identifying the errors that help us then compensate for the errors and learn. Exactly. But it's very procedural. It's not about just listening and regurgitating. You've put your finger on it, which is that, and this is what we teach the students, is that you have to, the way the brain works, right, is not, it doesn't memorize things like a computer.
But it has to be active learning. You have to actively engage. In fact, when you're trying to solve a problem on your own, this is where you're really learning by trial and error, and that's a procedural system. But if someone tells you what the right answer is,
You know, that's just something that is a fact that it gets stored away somewhere, but it's not going to automatically come up if you actually are faced with something that's not exactly the same problem, but it's similar. And by the way, this is the key to AI, completely essential for the recent success of these large language models, you know, that the public now is beginning to use, is that they're not parrots. They just don't memorize what they've
the data that they've taken in, they have to generalize. That means to be able to do well on new things that come in that are similar to the old things that you've seen, but allow you to solve new problems. That's the key to the brain. The brain is really, really good at generalizing. In fact, in many cases, you only need one example to generalize. Like going to a restaurant for the first time, there are a number of new interactions.
There might be a host or a hostess. You sit down at these tables you never sat at, somebody asks you questions, you read it, okay, maybe it's a QR code these days, but forever after you understand the process of going into a restaurant doesn't matter what the genre of food happens to be or what city, sitting inside or outside, you can pretty much work it out. Sit at the counter, sit outside, sit at the table. There are a number of key action steps.
that I think pretty much translate to everywhere. Unless you go to some super high-end thing or some super low-end thing where it's a buffet or whatever, you can start to fill in the blanks here. If I understand correctly, there's an action function that's learned from the knowledge and the experience. Exactly. And then where is that action function stored? Is it in one location in the brain or is it kind of an emergent property of multiple brain areas?
So that you're right at the cusp here of where we are in neuroscience right now. We don't know the answer to that question. In the past, it had been thought that the cortex had like
countries that each part of the cortex was dedicated to one function. And interestingly, you record for the neurons, and it certainly looks that way. In other words, there's a visual cortex in the back, and there's a whole series of areas, and then there's an auditory cortex here in the middle, and then the prefrontal cortex for social interaction. And so it looked really clear-cut that it's modular.
And now we're facing is we have a new way to record from neurons. Optically, we can record from tens of thousands, from dozens of areas simultaneously. And what we're discovering is that if you want to do any task, you're engaging not just the area that you might think has the input coming in the visual system, but the visual system is getting input from the motor system.
Right, in fact, you know, there's more input coming from the motor system than from the eye. Really? Yes. Yeah, Ann Churchland at UCLA has shown that in the mouse. This is, so now we're looking at global interactions between all these areas. And that's where real complex cognitive behaviors emerge. It's from those interactions. And now we have the tools for the first time to actually be able to see them in real time. And we're doing that now.
first on mice and monkeys, but we now can do this in humans. So I've been collaborating with a group at Mass General Hospital to record from people with epilepsy, and they have to have an operation for people who are drug resistant.
To be able to find out where it starts in the cortex, where it is initiated, where the seizure starts, and then to go in, you have to go in and record simultaneously from a lot of parts of the cortex for weeks until you find out where it is, and then you go in and you try to take it out, and often that helps.
very, very invasive. But for two weeks, we have access to all those neurons in that cortex that are being recorded from constantly. And so I've used, I started out because I was interested in sleep. I wanted to understand what happens in the cortex of a human during sleep. But then we realized that you can also figure people who have these debilitating problems with seizures
You know, they're there for two weeks and they have nothing to do. So they just love the fact that scientists are interested in helping them and, you know, teaching them things and finding out where in the cortex things are happening when they learn something. This is a gold mine. It's unbelievable. And I've learned things from humans that I could have never gotten from any other species. Amazing. Obviously, language is one of them. But there are other things in sleep that we've we discovered.
having to do with traveling waves. There are circular traveling waves that go on during sleep, which is astonishing. Nobody ever really saw that before. If you were to ascribe one or two major functions to these traveling waves, what do you think they are accomplishing for us in sleep? And by the way, are they associated with deep sleep, slow wave sleep, or with rapid eye movement, sleep or both? This is non-REM sleep. This is a jargon. This is during intermediate.
transition states. Our audience will probably be keeping up. They've heard a lot about slow wave sleep from me and that Walker. This is a light slow wave sleep. Yeah. And so what are these traveling waves accomplished for us? Okay. So in the case of the, they're called sleep spindles. They last, the waves last for about
a second or two and they travel, like I say, in a circle around the cortex. It's known that these spindles are important for consolidating experiences you've had during the day into your long-term memory storage.
So it's a very important function. And if you take out, see, it's the hippocampus that is replaying the experiences. It's a part of the brain is very important for long-term memory. If you don't have a hippocampus, you can't learn new things. It doesn't say you can't remember what you did yesterday, or for that matter, even an hour earlier.
But the hippocampus plays back your experiences, causes the sleep spindles now to need that into the cortex. And it's important you do that right because you don't want to overwrite the existing knowledge you have. You just want to basically incorporate the new experience into your existing knowledge base in an efficient way that doesn't interfere with what you already know. So that's an example of a very important function that these traveling ways have.
I'd like to take a quick break and acknowledge our sponsor, AG1. AG1 is a vitamin mineral probiotic drink that includes prebiotics and adaptogens. I've been drinking AG1 since 2012, and I started doing it at a time when my budget was really limited. In fact, I only had enough money to purchase one supplement, and I'm so glad that I made that supplement, AG1.
The reason for that is even though I strive to eat whole foods and unprocessed foods, it's very difficult to get enough vitamins and minerals, micronutrients, and adaptogens from diet alone in order to make sure that I'm at my best, meaning have enough energy for all the activities I participate in from morning until night, sleeping well at night, and keeping my immune system strong.
When I take AG1 daily, I find that all aspects of my health, my physical health, my mental health, my performance, recovery from exercise, all of those improve. And I know that because I've had lapses when I didn't take my AG1 and I certainly felt the difference. I also noticed, and this makes perfect sense, given the relationship between the gut microbiome and the brain, that when I regularly take AG1, that I have more mental clarity and more mental energy.
If you'd like to try AG1, you can go to drinkagone.com slash Huberman to claim a special offer. For this month only, November 2024, AG1 is giving away a free one-month supply of omega-3 fatty acids from fish oil, in addition to their usual welcome kit of five free travel packs and a year supply of vitamin D3K2.
As I've discussed many times before on this podcast, Omega-3 fatty acids are critical for brain health, mood, cognition, and more. Again, go to drinkAG1.com slash Huberman to claim this special offer. Today's episode is also brought to us by David.
David makes a protein bar unlike any other. It has 28 grams of protein, only 150 calories and 0 grams of sugar. That's right, 28 grams of protein and 75% of its calories come from protein. These bars from David also taste amazing. My favorite flavor is chocolate chip cookie dough. But then again, I also like the chocolate fudge flavored one.
and I also like the cake flavored one. Basically, I like all the flavors. They're incredibly delicious. For me personally, I strive to eat mostly whole foods. However, when I'm in a rush, or I'm away from home, or I'm just looking for a quick afternoon snack, I often find that I'm looking for a high quality protein source. With David, I'm able to get 28 grams of protein with the calories of a snack, which makes it very easy to hit my protein goals of one gram of protein per pound of body weight each day. And it allows me to do that without taking in excess calories.
I typically eat a David Bar in the early afternoon or even mid afternoon if I want to bridge that gap between lunch and dinner. I like that it's a little bit sweet, so it tastes like a tasty snack, but it's also given me that 28 grams of very high quality protein with just 150 calories. If you would like to try David, you can go to davidprotein.com slash Huberman. Again, the link is davidprotein.com slash Huberman.
As I recall, there are one or two things that one can do in order to ensure that one gets sufficient sleep spindles at night and thereby incorporate this new knowledge. This was from the episode that we did with Gina Poe.
from UCLA, I believe, and others, including Matt Walker. My recollection is that the number one thing is to make sure you get enough sleep at night, so you experience enough of these spindles. And we're all familiar with the cognitive challenges, including memory challenges and learning challenges associated with lack of sleep, insufficient sleep. But the other was that there was some interesting relationship between daytime exercise and nighttime prevalence of sleep spindles. Are you familiar with that literature?
No, this is a fascinating literature. And it's all pointing in the same direction, which is that we always neglect to appreciate the importance of sleep. I mean, obviously, you're refreshed when you wake up. But there's a lot of things happening. It's not that your brain turns off. It goes into a completely different state. And memory consolidation is just one of those things that happens when you're
fall asleep. And of course, you know, there's dreams and so forth. We don't fully appreciate or understand exactly how all the different sleep stages are worked together. But exercise is a particularly important
part of getting the motor system tuned up. It's not that the REM, rapid eye movement sleep may be involved in that. That's yet another part of the sleep.
stages, you go through, you go back and forth between stream sleep and the slow way sleep, back and forth, back and forth during the night. And then when you wake up, you're in the REM stage, more and more REM, more and more REM.
But that's all observation. But as a scientist, what you want to do is perturb the system and see if you had more sleep spindles, maybe you'd be able to remember things better. So it turns out, Sarah Mednick, who was at UC Irvine, did this fantastic experiment. So it turns out there's a drug called Zolpiedem, which
It goes by the name Ambien. You may have some experience with that. I've never taken it, but I'm aware of what it is. People use it as a sleep aid. That's right. A lot of people take it in order to sleep. OK. Well, it turns out that it causes more sleep spindles. Really? Yeah. It doubles the number of sleep spindles. If you take the drug,
After you've done the learning, right, you do the learning at night and then you take the drug and you have twice as many spindles, you wake up in the morning, you can remember twice as much.
what you learned. And the memories are stable over time. It's in there. Yeah, no, it consolidates it. I mean, that's the point. What's the downside of ambient? Okay, here's the downside. Okay, so people who take the drug say if you're going to Europe and you take it and then you sleep really soundly, but often you
You find yourself in the hotel room and you completely have no clue. You have no memory of how you got there. I've had that experience without Ambien or any other drugs where I am very badly jet-lagged. Yes. And I wake up in for a few seconds, but what feels like eternity, I have no idea where I am. It's terrifying. Well, that's another problem that you have with jet lag. Jet lag really screws things up. But this is something where it could be an hour.
You know, you took the train or you took a taxi or something. And so here, this seems crazy. How could it be a way to improve learning and recall on one hand and then forgetfulness on the other hand? Well, it turns out what's important is that when you take the drug,
Right. In other words, it helps consolidate experiences you've had in the past before you took the drug.
but it'll wipe out experiences you have in the future after you take the drug, right? So I'm not laughing, it must be a terrifying experience, but I'm laughing because there's some beautiful pharmacology and indeed some wonderfully useful pharmaceuticals out there. Some people may cringe to hear me say that, but there are some very useful drugs out there that save lives and help people deal with symptoms, et cetera. Side effects are always a concern, but this particular drug profile, Ambien,
That is, it seems to reveal something perhaps even more important than the discussion about spindles or ambient or even sleep, which is that you got to pay the piper somehow, as they say. That's right. You tweak one thing in the brain, something else goes. You don't get anything for free.
That's true. I think that this is something that is true, not just of drugs for the brain, but steroids for the body. Sure. Yeah. I mean, steroids, even low dose testosterone therapy, which is very popular nowadays, will give people more vigor, et cetera. But it is introducing a sort of second puberty. And puberty is perhaps the most rapid phase of aging.
of the entire lifespan. Same thing with people who take growth hormone would be probably a better example. Because certainly those therapies can be beneficial to people, but growth hormone gives people more vigor, but it accelerates aging. Look at the quality of skin that people have when they take growth hormone. It looks more age. They physically change. And I'm not for or against these things. It's highly individual. But I completely agree with you. I would also venture that
with the growing interest in so-called neutropics and people taking things like modafinil, not just for narcolepsy, daytime sleepiness, but also to enhance cognitive function. Okay, maybe they can get away with doing that every once in a while for a deadline task or something, but my experience is that people who obsess over the use of pharmacology to achieve certain brain states pay in some other way. Absolutely. Whether or not stimulants or sedatives or sleep drugs and that behaviors will always prevail.
behaviors will always prevail as tools. Yeah. And one of the things about the way the body evolved is that it really has to balance a lot of things. And so with drugs, you're basically unbalancing it somehow.
And the consequence is, as you point out, is that in order to make one part better, one part of your body, you sacrifice something else somewhere else. As long as we're talking about brain states and connectivity across areas, I want to ask a particular question. Then I want to return to this issue about how best to learn, especially in kids, but also in adulthood. I've become very interested in and spent a lot of time with the literature and some guests on the topic of psychedelics.
Let's leave the discussion about LSD aside, because do you know why there aren't many studies of LSD? This is kind of a fun one. No one is expected to know the answers. Well, it's against the law, I think. Oh, but there's, so is psilocybin or MDMA, and there are lots of studies going on about this. Oh, there are no. Yeah, it's changed. But when I was growing up, you know, as you know, it was against the law. Right. So what I learned is that there are far fewer clinical trials exploring the use of LSD as a therapeutic, because with the exception of Switzerland, none of the researchers are willing to stay in the laboratory as long as it takes for the
subject to get to an LSD journey, whereas psilocybin tends to be a shorter experience. Okay. Let's talk about psilocybin for a moment. My read of the data on psilocybin is that it's still open to question, but that some of the clinical trials show pretty significant recovery from major depression. It's pretty impressive, but if we just set that aside and say, okay, more needs to be worked out for safety, what is very clear from the brain imaging studies?
or before and after, resting state, task related, et cetera, is that you get more resting state global connectivity, more areas talking to more areas than was the case prior to the use of the psychedelic. And given the similarity of the psychedelic journey, and here specifically talking about psilocybin, to things like rapid eye movement, sleep, and things of that sort, I have a very simple question. Do you think that there's any real benefit
to increasing brain wide connectivity. To me, it seems a little bit haphazard, and yet the clinical data are promising, if nothing else, promising. And so, is what we're seeking in life as we acquire new knowledge
as we learn tennis or golf or take up singing or what have you, as we go from childhood into the late stages of our life, that whole transition is what we're doing, increasing connectivity and communication between different brain areas. Is that what the human experience is really about?
Or is it that we're getting more modular? We're getting more segregated in terms of this area, talking to this area in this particular way. Feel free to explore this in any way that feels meaningful. Or to say pass if it's not a good question.
No, it's a great question. I mean, you have all these great questions, and we don't have complete answers yet. But specifically with regard to connectivity, if you look at what happens in an infant's brain during the first two years, there's a tremendous amount of new synapses being formed. This is your area, by the way. You know about this, and I do. But then you prune them.
Right? The second phase is that you overabundant synapses, and that what you want to do is to prune them. Why would you want to do that? Well,
Synapses are expensive. It takes a lot of energy to activate all of the neurons and the synapses, especially, because it's the turnover of the neurotransmitter. And so what you want to do is to reduce the amount of energy and only use those synapses that have been proven to be the most important. Now, unfortunately, as you get older,
The pruning slows down but doesn't go away. So the cortex thins and so forth. So I think it goes in the opposite direction. I think that as you get older, you're losing connectivity. But interestingly, you retain the old memories. The old memories are really rock solid because they were put in when you were young. Yeah, the foundation. The foundation upon which everything else is built.
But it's not totally one way in the sense that
Even as an adult, as you know, you can learn new things. Maybe not as quickly. By the way, this is one of the things that surprised me. So Barbara and I have looked at the people who really were the benefit of the most. It turns out that the peak of the demographic is 25 to 35. Barbara. Oakley. Oakley. Yeah, she's really the mastermind. She's a fabulous educator and background in engineering.
But what's going on? So it turns out we aimed our MOOC at kids in high school and college because that's their business. They go every day and they go into work. They have to learn, right? That's their business.
But in fact, very few of the students who were actually, you know, they weren't taking the court. Why should they? They spent all day in the class, right? Why do they want to take another class? So this is the learning to learn class. Learning how to learn. Okay. So you did this with Barbara. So we did this, I did with Barbara. And now 25 to 35, we have this huge peak, huge. So what's going on? Here's what's going on. It's very interesting.
So you're 25. You've gone to college. You have to people, by the way, who take the course, went to college, right? So it's not like filling in for colleges is like topping it off. But you're in the workforce.
you have to learn new skill. Maybe you have mortgage, maybe you have children, right? You can't afford to go off and take a course of, or get another degree. So you take a MOOC and you discover, I'm not quite as agile as I used to be in terms of learning, but it turns out with our course, you can boost your learning
And so that even though you're not as, your brain isn't learning as quickly, you can do it more efficiently. This is amazing. I wanna take this course. I will take this course. What sort of time commitment is the course? You already pointed out that it's zero cost, which is amazing. Yeah, yeah, okay. So it's bite-sized videos lasting about 10 minutes each, and there's about 50 or 60 over a course of one month.
And are you tested, are you self-tested? Yeah, there are tests, there are quizzes, there are tests at the end. And there are forums where you can go and talk to other students, you have questions, we have TAs. No, it's- And anyone can do this. Anyone in the world, in fact, we have people in India, housewives, who say, thank you, thank you, thank you, because I could have never learned about how to be a better learner. And I wish I had known this when I was going to school. Why do more people not know about this learning to learn the course?
As people know, if I get really excited about it or about anything, I'm never going to shut up about it. Well, but I'm going to take the course first because I want to understand the guts of it. You know, you'll enjoy it. We have like 98% approval. It's just phenomenal. It's sticky. Is it math vocabulary? No, no math. We're not teaching anything specific. We're not trying to give you knowledge. We're trying to tell you how to acquire knowledge.
and how to do that, how to deal with exam anxiety, for example, or how to, you know, we all procrastinate, right? We put things off. No, no, I'm kidding. We all procrastinate. How to avoid that? We teach you how to avoid that. Fantastic. Okay, I'm going to skip back a little bit now with the intention of double clicking on this learning to learn thing. You pointed out that
in particular in California but elsewhere as well. There isn't as much procedural practice based learning anymore. I'm going to play devil's advocate here and I'm going to point out that this is not what I actually believe but
You know, when I was growing up, you had to do your times tables and your division and then your fractions and your exponents. And they build on one another. And then at some point, you take courses where you might need a graph and calculator to some people that can be like, what is this? But the point being that there were a number of things that you had to learn to implement functions and you learn by doing.
you learn by doing. Likewise, in physics class, we were attaching things to strings for macro mechanics and learning that stuff. And learning from the chalkboard lectures.
I can see the value of both, certainly. And you explained that the brain needs both to really understand knowledge and how to implement and back and forth. But nowadays, you'll hear the argument, well, why should somebody learn how to read a paper map unless it's the only thing available because you have Google Maps? Or if they want to do a calculation, they just put it into the top bar function on the internet and boom, out comes the answer.
There is a world where certain skills are no longer required and one could argue that the brain space and activity and time and energy in particular could be devoted to
learning new forms of knowledge that are going to be more practical in the school and workforce going forward. So how do we reconcile these things? I mean, I'm over the belief that the brain is doing math and you and I agree. It's electrical signals and chemical signals and it's doing math and it's running algorithms. I think you convinced us of that, certainly. But how are we to discern what we need to learn versus what we don't need to learn in terms of building a brain that's capable of learning
the maximum number of things or even enough things so that we can go into this very uncertain future because as far as you know, and I know there's no, neither of us have a crystal ball. So what is essential to learn? And for those of us that didn't learn certain things in our formal education, what should we learn how to learn? Well, this is generational. Okay.
Technologies provide us with tools. You mentioned the calculator, right? Well, a calculator didn't eliminate
the education you need to get in math, but it made certain things easier. It made it possible for you to do more things, and more accurately. However, interestingly, students in my class often come up with answers that are off by eight orders of magnitude, and that's a huge amount, right? It's clear that they didn't key in the calculator properly, but they didn't recognize that it was a very
was completely way off the beam because they didn't have a good feeling for the numbers. They don't have a good sense of, you know, exactly how big it should have been, you know, order of magnitude, basic, you know, understanding. So it's kind of a
The benefit is that you can do things faster, better, but then you also lose some of your intuition if you don't have the procedural system in place. I'm thinking about a kid that wants to be a musician who uses AI to write a song about a bad breakup that then is
kind of recovered when they find new love. And I'm guessing that you could do this today and get a pretty good song out of AI. But would you call that kid a songwriter or a musician? On the face of it, yeah?
the AI is helping. And then you'd say, well, that's not the same as sitting down with a guitar and trying out different chords and, and feeling the intonation in their voice. But I'm guessing that for people that were on the electric guitar, they were criticizing people on the acoustic guitar. You know, so we have this generational thing where we look back and say, that's not the real thing. You need to get the, so what are the key fundamentals is really a critical question. Okay. So I'm going to come back to that because this is how you put it at the beginning.
had to do with how your brain is allocating resources. When you're younger, you can take in things. Your brain is more malleable. For example, how good are you on social media?
I will, I do all my own Instagram and Twitter and those accounts have grown in proportion to the amount of time I've been doing it. So yeah, I would say pretty good. I mean, I'm not the biggest account on social media, but for a science health account, we're doing okay. I'm thanks to the audience. Well, this speaks well for the fact that you've managed to break, you know, to go beyond the generation gap. I can type with my thumbs, Terry.
Okay, there you go. That's a manual skill to learn. That's a new phenomenon of human evolution. I couldn't believe it. I saw people doing that and now I can do it too. But the thing is that if you learn how to do that early in life, you're much more good at it. You can move your thumbs much more quickly. Also,
You can have many more You know tweets going and we're not where they called now. They're not called tweets on X I think they still call them tweets because you can't it's hard to verb the Word the letter X You London think of that one. I like X because it's cool. It's kind of punk and it's got black black Kind of format and it fits with it kind of the it's that the you know the engineer like black X You know and that's kind of thing, but but yeah that we'll still call them tweets
Well, okay, well, calm tweets. Okay, that's good. But, you know, I walk across campus and I see everybody, like half the people are tweeting.
or you know they're they're doing something with their cell phone there there i mean it's unbelievable you have beautiful sunsets at the salk institute will put a link to one of them i mean it is it is truly spectacular awe-inspiring to see a sunset at the salk it every day is different and everyone's on their phones these days sad and you know they're looking down at their phone and walking along even people who are skateboarding
Unbelievable. It's amazing what the human being can do when they learn to get into something. But what happens is the younger generation picks up whatever technology it is and the brain gets really good at it.
And you can pick it up later, but you're not quite as agile, not quite as maybe obsessive. It fatigues me. I will point this out. That doing anything on my phone feels fatiguing in a way that reading a paper book or even just writing on a laptop or a desktop computer is fundamentally different. I can do that for many hours. If I'm on social media for more than a few minutes, I can literally feel the energy draining out of my body. Interesting. I could do,
sprints or deadlifts for hours and not feel the kind of fatigue that I feel from doing social media. So, you know, this is fascinating. I'd like to know what's going on in your brain. Why is it? And also, I'd like to know from younger people whether they have the same. I think not. I think my guess is that they don't feel fatigued because they got into this early enough. And this is actually a very, very, I think that has a lot to do with
the foundation you put into your brain. In other words, things that you learn when you're really young
are foundational and they make things easier, some things easier. I spent a lot of time in my room as a kid either playing with Legos or action figures or building fish tanks or reading about fish. I tended to read about things and then do a lot of procedural based activities. I would read skateboard magazines and skateboard. I was never one to really just watch a sport and not play it.
So, you know, bridging across these things. So social media, to me, feels like an energy sink. But of course, I love the opportunity to be able to teach to people and learn from people at such scale. But at an energetic level, I feel like I don't have a foundation for it. It's like I'm trying to, like Jerry rigged my cognition into doing something that it wasn't designed to do.
Well, there you go. And it's because you don't have the foundation. You didn't do it when you were younger. And now you have to sort of use the cognitive powers to do a lot of what was being done now in a younger person procedurally.
I'd like to take a quick break and thank one of our sponsors, Element. Element is an electrolyte drink that has everything you need and nothing you don't. That means the electrolytes, sodium, magnesium, and potassium in the correct ratios, but no sugar. We should all know that proper hydration is critical for optimal brain and body function.
In fact, even a slight degree of dehydration can diminish your cognitive and physical performance to a considerable degree. It's also important that you're not just hydrated, but that you get adequate amounts of electrolytes in the right ratios. Drinking a packet of element dissolved in water makes it very easy to ensure that you're getting adequate amounts of hydration and electrolytes. To make sure that I'm getting proper amounts of both, I dissolve one packet of element in about 16 to 32 ounces of water when I wake up in the morning, and I drink that basically first thing in the morning.
I'll also drink a packet of element dissolved in water during any kind of physical exercise that I'm doing, especially on hot days when I'm sweating a lot and losing water and electrolytes. There are a bunch of different grade tasting flavors of element. I like the watermelon, I like the raspberry, I like the citrus, basically I like all of them.
If you'd like to try element, you can go to drinkelement.com slash Huberman to claim an element sample pack with the purchase of any element drink mix. Again, that's drinkelement spelled L-M-N-T. So it's drinkelement.com slash Huberman to claim a free sample pack.
Today's episode is also brought to us by JUVE. JUVE makes medical grade red light therapy devices. Now, if there's one thing that I've consistently emphasized on this podcast is the incredible impact that light can have on our biology. Now, in addition to sunlight, red light and near infrared light have been shown to have positive effects on improving numerous aspects of cellular and organ health.
including faster muscle recovery, improved skin health and wound healing, improvements in acne, reduced pain and inflammation, improved mitochondrial function, and even improving vision itself. Now, what sets Juve lights apart and why they're my preferred red light therapy devices is that they use clinically proven wavelengths, meaning they use specific wavelengths of red light and urine for red light in combination to trigger the optimal cellular adaptations. Personally, I use the Juve whole body panel about three to four times a week,
and I use the JUVE Handheld Light both at home and when I travel. If you'd like to try JUVE, you can go to JUVE spelled J-O-O-V-V dot com slash Huberman. JUVE is offering Black Friday discounts of up to $1,300 now through December 2nd, 2024. Again, that's JUVE J-O-O-V-V dot com slash Huberman to get up to $1,300 off select JUVE products.
I'm going to tell you something which is going to help all of your listeners. My book, Chat GDP and the Future of AI, I went through and I looked at other people's experiences with Chat GDP. I just wanted to know what people were thinking and what, and I came across, it was an article and I think it was the New York Times of a technical writer who decided she'd spend one month using it to help her write things, her articles. And she said that when she started out,
You know, at the end of the day, she was drained, completely drained. And it was like, you know, working on a machine, you know, like a tractor or something, you know, you're just struggling, struggling, struggling to get it to work. And then she started, said, well, wait a second, you know, what if I treated like a human being? What if I'm polite instead of, you know, being curt? She said, suddenly,
I started getting better answers by being polite and back and forth the way you were with the human. So saying, could you please give me information about so-and-so? Yeah, please. I'm really having trouble. That answer you gave me was fabulous, is exactly what I was looking for. And now I need you to go on to the next part and help me with that, too. In other words, the way you talk to a human, if you're an assistant. Or is it that she was talking to the AI to chat GPT, it sounds like, in this case,
in the way that her brain was familiar with asking questions to a human. In other words, is the AI learning her and therefore giving her the sorts of answers that are more facile for her to integrate with? I think it's both.
Well, first of all, the chat GDP is mirroring the way you treat it. It will mirror that back. You treat it like a machine. It will treat you like a machine, okay? Because that's what it's good at. But here's the surprise. Surprise is. She said, once I started treating it like a human, at the end of the day, I wasn't fatigued anymore.
Why? Well, it turns out that all your life you interact with humans in a certain way and your brain is wired to do that and it doesn't take any effort. And so by treating the chat GDP as if we're a human, you're taking advantage of all the brain circuits in your brain.
This is incredible. And I'll tell you why, because I think many people, not just me, but many people, really enjoy social media. Learn from it. Yesterday, I learned a few things that I thought were just fascinating about how we perceive our own identity, according to whether or not we're filtering it through the responses of others or whether or not we take a couple of minutes and really just sit and think about how we actually feel about ourselves. Very interesting ideas about locus of self-perception and things like that.
I also looked at a really cool video of a baby raccoon popping bubbles while standing on its hind limbs. And that was really cool, and social media could provide me both those things within the series of minutes. And I was thinking to myself, this is crazy, right? The raccoon is kind of trivial, but it delighted me, and that's not trivial.
But here's the question. Could it be that one of the detrimental aspects of social media is that if we're complimenting one another, or if we are giving hearts, or we're giving thumbs down, or we're in an argument with somebody, or we're doing a clap back, or they're clapping back on us as it, or dunking as it's called on X, huh?
that it isn't necessarily the way that we learned to argue. It's not necessarily the way that we learned to engage in healthy dispute. And so as a consequence, it feels like, and this is my experience, that certain online interactions feel really good, and others feel like they're kind of great on me. Because there's almost like an action step that isn't allowed, like you can't fully explain yourself or understand the other person.
And I am somebody who believes in the power of real face-to-face dialogue or at least on the phone dialogue. And I feel the same way about text messaging. I hate text messaging. When text messaging first came out, I remember thinking, I was not a kid that passed notes in class. This feels like passing notes in class. In fact, this whole text messaging thing is beneath me.
That's how I felt. And over the years, of course, I became a text messenger. And it's very useful for certain things, be there in five minutes, running a few minutes late. In my case, that's a common one. But I think this notion of what greats on us and as it relates to whether or not it matches our childhood developed template of how our brain works is really key because it touches on something that I definitely want to talk about today that I know you've worked on quite a bit, which is this concept of energy.
What we're talking about here is energy, not woo biology, woo science, wellness, energy. We're talking about, we only have a finite amount of energy. And years ago, the great Ben Barris sadly passed away, our former colleague and my postdoc advisor came to me one day in the hallway and he stopped me and he said, he called me Andy like you do. And he said, Andy?
How can we get such a rundown of energy as we get older? Why am I more tired today than I was 10 years ago? I was like, I don't know, how are you sleeping? He's like, I'm sleeping fine. Ben never slept much in the first place, but he had a ton of energy. And I thought to myself, I don't know.
Like, what is this energy thing that we're talking about? I want to make sure that we close the hatch on this notion of a template neural system that then you either find experiences invigorating or depleting. I want to make sure we close the hatch on that. But I want to make sure that we relate it at some point to this idea of energy. And why is it that with each passing year of our life, we seem to have less of it?
You asked these great questions, and I wish that I had great answers. Well, so far you really do have great answers. There's certainly novel to me in the sense that I've not heard answers of this sort. So there's a tremendous amount of learning for me today, and I know for the audience. But let's say somebody is 20 years old versus 50 years old versus what should they do? I mean, we need to integrate with the modern world. We also need to relate across generations.
Oh yeah, no, this is true. This is true. You're retiring as much. They're living longer. Birth rates are down, but we have to get all, get along as they say. So, you know, it is interesting. And I think it's true that we all, as we get older, have less of the, you know, the vigor, vigor, if I could use this so much different word from energy. We'll come back to that. But I think there are some who manage to keep an active life.
something that, again, in our MOOC, we really emphasize. Could you explain a MOOC? I think most people won't know what a MOOC is, just for their sake. They've been around for about, actually started at Stanford, Andrew Ng and Daphne Kohler, so they have a company called Crossera.
And what happens is that you get professors, and in fact, anybody who has knowledge or professional expertise to give lectures that are available to anybody in the world who have access to the internet. And there's probably tens of thousands now. Any specialty of history,
science, music, you name it. There's somebody who's an expert on that and wants to tell you because they're excited about what they're doing. What we wanted to do was to help people with learning. Part of the problem is that it gets more difficult. It takes more effort as you get older.
It depletes your vigor more if we're going to stay with this language of energy and vigor. Yeah, that's right. So let's actually use the word energy. As you know, in the cell, there is a physical power plant called the mitochondrion, which is supplying us with ATP, which is the coin of the realm for the cell to be able to operate all of its machinery. And so one of the things that happens when you get older is that your mitochondrial run down.
You have fewer of them and they're less efficient. That's right, they're less efficient. And actually drugs can do that too too, because they can harm mitochondria. Recreational drugs. No, the drugs you take for illness. I'm not sure about recreational drugs, but I know the case that there are a lot of drugs that people take because they have to.
But the other thing, and this is something, that's the bad news, here's the good news. The good news is that you can replenish your energy by exercise. Exercise is the best drug you could ever take. It's the cheapest drug you could ever take that can help every organ in your body.
It helps, obviously, your heart. It helps your brain. It rejuvenates your brain. It helps your immune system. Every single organ system in the body benefits from a regular exercise. I run on the beach every day.
at the Salk Institute. And I also at the, it's on a Mesa, 340 foot above the, so I go down every day and then I climb up the cliff. Yeah, those steps down to Black's Beach are, they're a good workout. They are, they are. And so this is something has kept me active and I do hiking and when hiking in the Alps in last fall.
So this is in September. So this is, I think, something that people really ought to realize is that, you know, it's like putting away, you know, reserves of energy for, you know, when you get older, the more you put away, the better off you are. Here's something else. Okay, now this is jumping now to Alzheimer's.
So a study that was done in China many, many years ago, when I first came to La Jolla San Diego, I heard this from the, it was the head of the Alzheimer's program. He had done a study in China on onset. And they went and they had three populations. They had peasants who had almost no education.
Then they had another group that had high school education, and they were people who were advanced education. So it turns out that the onset of Alzheimer's was earlier for the people who had no education. And it was the latest for the people who had the most education.
Now this is interesting, isn't it? And presumably the genes aren't that different, right? I mean, they're all Chinese. So one possibility, and obviously we don't really know why, but one possibility is that the more you exercise your brain with education, the more reserve you have.
later in life. I believe in the notion, and I don't have a better word for it, maybe you do, or a phrase for it, is of a cognitive velocity.
You know, I sometimes will play with this. I'll read slowly or I'll see where my default pace of reading is at a given time of day. And then I'll intentionally try and read a little bit faster while also trying to retain the knowledge I'm reading. So I'm not just reading the words. I'm trying to absorb the information. And you can feel the energetic demand of that. And then I'll play with it. I'll kind of back off a little bit.
And then I'll go forward. And I try and find the sweet spot where I'm not reading at the pace that is reflexive, but just a little bit quicker while also trying to retain the information. And I learned this when I had a lot of catching up to do at one phase of my educational career. Fortunately, it was pretty early, and I was able to catch up on most things, occasionally things slipped through, and I have to go back and learn how to learn. And if I get anything wrong on the internet, they sure as heck.
pointed out and then we go back and learning. Guess what? I'd never forget that because punishment, social punishment is a great signal. So thank you all for keeping me learning. But I picked that up from my experience of trying to get good at things like skateboarding or soccer when I was younger. There's a certain thing that happens when skateboarding. That was my sport growing up where it's actually easier to learn something going faster.
You know, most kids try and learn how to ollie and kickflip standing in the living room on the carpet. That's the worst way to learn how to do it. It's all easier going a bit faster than you're comfortable. It's also the case that if you're not paying attention, you can get hurt. It's also the case that if you pay too much cognitive attention, you can't perform the motor movements. So there's this sweet spot that eventually I was able to translate into an understanding of when I sit down to read a paper or a news article,
or even listen to a podcast, there's a pace of the person's voice and then I'll adjust the rate of the audio where I have to engage cognitively and I know I'm in a mode of retaining the information and learning. Whereas if I just go with my reflexive pace, it's rare that I'm in that perfect zone. So I point this out because perhaps it will be useful to people. I don't know if it's incorporated into your learning how to learn course, but I do think that there is something
which I call cognitive velocity, which is ideal for learning versus kind of leisurely scrolling. And this is why I think that social media is detrimental. I think that we train our brain basically to be slow, passive, and multi-context cycling through. And unless something is very high, salience.
It kind of makes us kind of fat and lazy, forgive the language, but I'm gonna be blunt here, fat and lazy cognitively, unless we make it a point to also engage learning. And my guess is it's tapping into this mitochondrial system. Very likely, that's one part of it. By the way, the way that you've adjusted the speed is very interesting, because it turns out that stress, everybody thinks our stress is bad, but no, it turns out stress.
that is transient, that is only for a limited amount of time, that you control is good for you, is good for your brain, is good for your body. I run intervals on the beach just the way that you do cognitive intervals when you're reading. In other words, I run like hell for about 10 seconds, and then I go to a jog and I run like hell for another 10 seconds, and it's pushing your body into that.
extra gear that helps the muscles, the muscles need to know that this is what they've got to put out. And that's where you gain muscle mass, not from just doing the same running pace every day.
Well, your intellectual and physical vigor is undeniable. I've known you a long time. You've always had a slight forward center of mass in your intellect and even the speed at which you walk, Terry, dare I say, you're for a Californian, you're a quick walker. Okay. Yeah. So that's a compliment, by the way. East coasters know what I'm talking about and Californians will be like, you know, why not slow down? The reason to not slow down too much for too long is that
these mitochondrial systems, the energy of the brain and body as you point out are very linked. And I do think that below a certain threshold, it makes it very hard to come back just like below a certain threshold is hard to exercise without getting very depleted or even injured. That we need to maintain this. So perhaps now will be a good time to close the hatch on this issue of how to teach young people. Everyone should take this learning to learn course as a free resource. Amazing.
as it relates to AI. Do you think that young people end older people now? I'm 49, so I'll put myself in the older bracket. Should be learning how to use AI.
they are already learning how to use AI. And again, it's just like new technology comes along, who picks up first? It's the younger people and it's astonishing. They're using it a lot more than I use it almost every day, but I know a lot of students who basically, and by the way, it's like any other tool, it's a tool that you need to know how to use it.
Where do you suggest people start? So I have started using Claude AI. This was suggested to me by somebody expert in AI as an alternative to chat GPT. I don't have anything against chat GPT. But I'll tell you, I really like the
aesthetic of clawed AI. It's a bit of a softer beige aesthetic. It feels kind of apple-like. I like the apple brand. And it gives me answers. Maybe it's the font. Maybe it's the feel. Maybe this goes back to the example used earlier where I like clawed AI.
and I'm a big fan of it and they don't pay me to say this, I have never met them, I have no relationship to them except that it gives me answers in a bullet pointed format that feels very aesthetically easy to transfer that information into my brain or onto a page. So I like clawed AI, use chat GPT, how should people start to explore AI for sake of getting smarter, learning knowledge just for the sake of knowledge, having fun with it? What's the best way to do that?
Well, I think exactly what you did, which is there's now dozens and dozens of different chatbots out there. And different people will feel comfortable with one or the other chat. GDP is the first. So that's why it's kind of taken over a lot of the...
cognitive space, right? It's become like Kleenex, right? That word. That was why I use it as the first word in my book because it's iconic. But some of them, I have to say that, for example, there are some that are really much better math than others.
Google's Gemini recently did some fine tuning with what's called, you know, chain of reasoning. Another reason you go through a sequence of steps. And when you solve a math problem, you go through a sequence of a partial steps of doing, you know, fitting first finding out what's missing and then adding that. And it went from 20% correct to 80%.
Right? And those problems. And as people hear that, they probably think, well, that means 20% are wrong still, but could you imagine any human or panel of humans behind a wall where if you asked it a question and then another question and another question that it would give you back better than 80% accurate information in a matter of seconds? So I think we are
being perhaps a little bit unfair to compare these large language models to the best humans, rather than the average human, right? As you said, most people couldn't pass the LSAT, the loss test to get into law school, or MCAT, the test to get into medical school. And chat GPT has, is there a
world now where we take the existing AI, LLMs, these computers basically that can learn like a collection of human brains and send that somehow into the future, right? Give them an imagined future. Okay. Could we give them outcome A and outcome B and let them forage into
future states that we are not yet able to get to and then harness that knowledge and explore the two different outcomes. I think that's perhaps the better question in some sense because we can't travel back in time.
but we can perhaps travel into the future with AI if you provide it different scenarios. And you say, unlike a panel of people, panel of experts, medical experts or space travel experts or sea travel experts, you can't say, hey, you know what, don't sleep tonight. You're just gonna work for the next,
48 hours. In fact, you're going to work for the next three weeks or three months. Um, and you know what? You're not going to do anything else. You're not going to pay attention to your health. You're not going to do anything else, but you can take a large language model and you can say just forage for knowledge under the following different scenarios and then have that fleet of large language models come back and give us the information like, I don't know tomorrow. Okay. So I've lived through this myself back in the 1980s.
I was just starting my career, and I was one of the pioneers in developing learning algorithms for neural network models. Jeff Hinton and I collaborated together on something called the Bolsom Machine, and he actually won an Nobel Prize for this. Just this year. Yeah, he's one of my best friends. Brilliant, and he did well deserved it for not just the Bolsom Machine, but all the work he's done since then on machine learning and then back propagation and so forth. But back then,
We, Jeff and I, had this view of the future. AI was dominated by symbol processing, rules, logic, writing computer programs. For every problem, you need a different computer program. And it was very human resource intensive to write programs so that it was very, very slow going.
And they never actually got there. They never wrote a program for vision, for example, even though the computer vision, computer, the community really worked hard for a long time. But, you know, we had this view of the future. We had this view that the nature has solved these problems, even as existence proof that you can solve the vision problem. Look, every animal can see even insects, right? Come on. We'll figure out, let's figure out how they did it. Maybe we can help by following up on nature, we can actually, again, going back to algorithms, I was telling you.
And so in the case of the brain, what makes it different from a digital computer, digital computers basically can run any program, but a fly brain, for example, only runs the program that is a special purpose hardware, allows it to run. Not much neuroplasticity. There's enough there, just enough habituation and so forth, so that you can
survive, and this is, that's a wrap 24 hours. I'm not trying to be disparaging to the fly biologists, but when I think of neuroplasticity, I think of the magnificent neuroplasticity of the human brain to customize to a world of experience. I agree, but when I think about a fly, I think about a really cool set of neural circuits that work really well to avoid getting swatted, to eating, and to reproducing, and not a whole lot else.
They don't really build technology. They might have interesting relationships, but who knows who cares. It's just sort of like, it's not that it doesn't matter. It's just a question of the lack of plasticity makes them kind of a meh species. Okay, I can see I've pressed your button here.
No, no, no, no. I love fly biology. They taught us about algorithms for direction, selectivity, and the visual system. Oh, no, no. I love the Dersophila biology. I just think that the lack of neuroplasticity reveals a certain, like, key limitation. And the reason we're the curators of the Earth is because we have so much plasticity.
Of course, of course. But you have to, you know, one step at a time, nature first has to be able to create creatures that can survive and then, you know, their brains is bigger as the environment gets more complex and, you know, here we are. But the key is that
It turns out that certain algorithms in the fly brain are present in our brain, like conditioning, classical conditioning. You can classical condition a fly in terms of training it to when you give it a reward, it will produce the same action, right? This is like condition behavior.
And that algorithm that I told you about, that isn't your value function, right? Temporal difference learning. That algorithm is in the fly brain. It's in your brain. So we can learn about learning from many species. Okay. I was just having a little fun poking at the fly biologist. I actually think Trosophila has done a great deal as has honeybee biology. For instance, if you, if you give caffeine,
to bees on particular flowers, they'll actually try and pollinate those flowers more because they actually like the feeling of being caffeinated. There's a bad pun about a buzz here, but I'm not going to make that pun because everyone's done it before. No, I fully absorb and agree with the value of studying simpler organisms to find the algorithms.
Right, that's where we are right now. But now to just go into the future now. I'm telling the story about what we really were. We were predating the future.
We were saying, this is an alternative to traditional AI. We were not taken seriously. Everybody was, experts said, no, no, write programs, write programs. They were getting all the resources, the grants, the jobs. And we were just like the little furry mammals under the feet of these dinosaurs, right? In retrospect.
But the dinosaurs died off. But the point I'm making is that it's possible for our brain to make these extrapolations into the future. Why not AI? Versions of brains. Why not? I think your idea is a great one. I mean, the reason I'm excited about AI and increasingly so across the course of this conversation is because
there are very few opportunities to forage information at such large scale and around the circadian clock. I mean, if there's one thing that we are truly a slave to as humans is the circadian biology. You got to sleep sooner or later. And even if you don't, your cognition really waxes and wanes across the circadian cycle. And if you don't, you're going to die early. We know this. Computers can work, work, work.
uh... sure you gotta power them there's the cooling thing there are a bunch of things related to that but that's that's tractable so computers can work work work and the idea that they can provide a portal into the future and that they can just bring it back so we can take a look see i'm not saying we have to implement their their advice but to be able to send a panel of
diverse, computationally diverse, experientially diverse.
AI experts into the future and bring us back a panel of potential routes to take. To me is so exciting. Maybe a good example would be like treatments for schizophrenia. This is an area that I want to make sure that we talk about. You know, I grew up learning as a neuroscience student that schizophrenia was somehow a disruption of the dopamine system because if you give neuroelectric drugs that block dopamine receptors that you get some improvement in the
in the motor symptoms and some of the hallucinations, et cetera. You now also have people who say, no, that's not really the basis of schizophrenia. I'd love your thoughts. And you have incredible work from people like Chris Palmer at Harvard, and we even have a department at Stanford now.
focusing, we even have people at Stanford now focusing on what Chris really founded as a field, which is metabolic psychiatry, the idea that, who could imagine? I'm being sarcastic here. What you eat impacts your mitochondria, how you exercise impacts your mitochondria, mitochondria impacts brain function, and what would be hold?
metabolic health of the brain and body impacts schizophrenia symptoms. And he's looked at ways that people can use ketogenic diet, maybe not to cure, but to treat. And in some cases, maybe even cure schizophrenia. So here we are at this place where we still don't have a quote unquote cure for schizophrenia, but you could send
LLMs into the future and start to forage the most likely all of the data in those fields. Probably could do that in an hour. Plus, come up with a bunch of hypothesized different positive and negative result clinical trials that don't even exist yet.
10,000 subjects in Scandinavia who go on ketogenic diet, who have a certain level of susceptibility schizophrenia based on what we know from twin studies, things that never, ever, ever would be possible to do in an afternoon, maybe even in a year. There's this in funding.
boom, get the answers back and let them present us those answers. And then you say, well, it's artificial, but so are human brains coming up with these experiments. So to me, I'm starting to realize that it's not that we have to implement everything that AI tells us or offers us, but it sure as hell gives us a great window into what might be happening or is likely to happen.
Specifically for schizophrenia, I'm pretty sure that if we had these large language models 20 years ago, we would have known back then that ketamine would have been a really good drug to try to help these people. Tell us about the relationship between ketamine and schizophrenia, because I think a lot of people, and maybe you could define schizophrenia, even though most people think about people hearing voices and psychosis, there's a bit more to it that maybe we just
Okay, so one of the things now that we know, see, the problem is that if you look at the endpoint, that doesn't tell you what started the problem. It started during early in development, schizophrenia.
is something that appears when, you know, late adolescence, early adulthood, but it actually is already a problem, a genetic problem from the Pukefco. So what is the concordance in identical twins, meaning if you have one identical twin, if you have identical twins in the womb, and one is destined to be full-blown schizophrenic,
What's the probability of the other heavy? So here's the experiment. This has been replicated many, many times in mice, I should say. No, actually, let me start with the human. So ketamine is, for a long time, and it still is a party drug, special K. I've never taken it, but this is what I do. I don't know for that, but I'll tell you what happens, because I've talked to these people who've done this,
You take ketamine, subanesthetic, by the way, it's an anesthetic. It's given to children. It's a pretty good anesthetic, and it's also used veterinary medicine. But in any case, you give it to, you take young adults, here's what they experience. They experience, out of body experience, they have this wonderful feeling of energy, and it's a high, but it's a very unusual high.
Now, if they just go and have one experience, but if they have two, like they party two days in a row, a lot of them come into the emergency room. And here's what the symptoms are. Full-blown psychosis. Full-blown. We're talking about indistinguishable
from a schizophrenic break. So auditory hallucinations. Yeah, auditory hallucinations, paranoia, very, very advanced. You'd say that, my God, this person here has become a schizophrenic, and this is really, like you say, the symptoms are the same. However, if you isolate them for a couple of days, come back.
Right? So it means that schizophrenia can induce, I'm sorry, ketamine can't induce a form of schizophrenia psychosis, temporarily, not permanently, fortunately.
Okay, so what is it attack? Okay, and there's another literature on this. It turns out that it binds to a form of receptor, a glutamate receptor, called NMDA receptors, which are very important by way for learning and memory. But we know the target, and we also know what the acute outcome is that it
reduces the strength of the inhibitory circuit, the interneurons that use inhibitory transmitters. The enzyme that creates the inhibitory transmitter is downregulated. And what does that do? It means that there's more excitation. And what does that mean with more excitation? It means that there's more activity in the bacortex, and there's actually much more vigor, and you start becoming crazy, right, if it's too much activity.
So this is interesting. So this is telling us, I think, that we should be thinking about, and now there's a whole field now in psychiatry that has to do with the glutamate hypothesis for the first where the actual
imbalance first occurs. It's an imbalance between the excitatory inhibitory systems that are in the cortex are keep you in balance. And NMDA and methyl deaspartate receptors are glutamate receptors. Yes, they are. They're one class. That's one class. That's right. Okay, so now
Here is a hypothesis for why ketamine might be good for depression. People are taking it now. We're depressed, right? So here we have a drug that causes over-excitation. And here you have a person who is under-excited. Depression is associated with lower excitatory activity in some parts of the cortex.
Well, if you titrate it, you can come back into balance, right? So what you do is you fight depression with schizophrenia, touch of schizophrenia. Now, you have to keep giving, I think once every three weeks, they have to have a new dose of ketamine, but it's helped an enormous number of people with very, very severe clinical depression. So as we learn more about the mechanisms underlying some of these disorders, the better we are going to be
extrapolating and coming up with some solutions at least to prevent it from getting worse. By the way, I'm pretty sure that the large language models could have figured this out longer ago.
In an attempt to understand how we might be able to leverage these large language models now, how would we have used these large language models long ago? Let's say you had 2024 AI technology in 19. Let's have fun here. 1998, the year that I started graduate school.
Right. At that time, it was like the dopamine hypothesis is because the friendia was in every textbook. There was a little bit about glutamate, perhaps, but, you know, it was all about dopamine. So how would the large language models have discovered this? Ketamine was known as a drug. Ketamine, by the way,
is very similar to PCP, like Lydin, which also bonds the NMB receptor. So, which is also a part of... It was also, yeah, not one I recommend, nor ketamine. Frankly, I don't recommend any recreational drugs, but I'm not a recreational drug guy. But what would those large language models do if they... So you've got 2024 technology placed into 1998. They're foraging for existing knowledge.
But then are they able to make predictions? Like, hey, this stuff is going to turn out to be wrong, or hey, this stuff is- Okay, okay, okay. You know, this is all very, very speculative. And really, we can begin actually to see this happening now. So I have a colleague at the Salk Institute, Rusty Gage.
very distinguished neuroscientist. And he was, he was one of the, he discovered that there are new neurons being born in the hippocampus, right? Which is something in adults, which is something that in a textbook says that doesn't happen, right? So that was around 1998. That's right. And I actually have a paper with him where we tested LTP, long-term potentiation of
Actually, the effects of exercise on neurogenesis. Exercise increases neurogenesis. It increases the cells, it increases neurogenesis and also the cells that are become part of the circuit. More cells become integrated. And this is true in humans as well, right?
Yeah, we, and there was some cancer drug that was given that, you know, that they showed that it was their new cells that were able, that they were able to later in postmortem to actually see that they were born in the adult. Okay. So here we are. Okay. In 1998. And the question is, can you, can you jump? Can you jump into the future? Okay. So rusty.
We happened to talk about this issue about he's using these large language models now for his research. I said, wow, how do you use it? He said, we used it as an idea pump. What do you mean idea pump? Well, we give it all of the experiments that we've done.
and uh... and we have that you know that uh... the literature it's access to the literature and so forth and we ask it for ideas for nukes parable i love it i love it uh... i was on a plane where i sat next to a guy that worked it works at google and he he's one of the uh... main people there in terms of voice to to text
and text-to-voice software. And he showed me something. I'll provide a link to it because it's another one of these open resource things. And I'm not super techy. I'm not like the, I don't get an F in technology. I don't get an A plus. I'm kind of in the mail. So I think I'm pretty representative of the average listener for this podcast, presumably. What he showed me is that you can take, you open up this website and you can take PDFs.
or you could take URLs, so websites, website addresses, and you just place them in the margin. You literally just drag and drop them there. And then you can ask questions
And the AI will generate answers that are based on the content of whatever you put into this margin, those PDFs, those websites. And the cool thing is it references them so you know which article it came from. And then you can start asking it more sophisticated questions.
in the two examples of the effects of a drug, one being very strong and one being very weak. Which of these papers do you think is more rigorous?
based on subject number, but also kind of the strength of the findings. You know, pretty vague things. Strength of findings is pretty vague, right? Anyone that argues, those are weak findings, those aren't enough subjects. Well, we know a hell of a lot about human memory from one patient, HM. So strength of findings when people is a subjective thing. You really have to be an expert in a field to understand strength of findings, and even that. And what's amazing is it starts giving back answers
Like, well, if you're concerned about number of subjects, this paper, but that's a pretty obvious one, which one had more subjects, but it can start critiquing these statistics that they used in these papers in very sophisticated ways and explain back to you why certain papers may not be interesting and others are more interesting and it starts to weight the evidence. And then you say, well, with that weighted evidence, can you
hypothesize what would happen if. And so I've done a little bit of this where it starts trying to predict the future based on, you know, 10 papers that you gave it five minutes ago. Amazing. I don't think any professor could do that except in their very specific area of interest and if they were already familiar with the papers and it would take them many hours if not days to read all those papers in detail.
And they might not actually come up with the same answers, right? Right. Yeah. So, so this is actually this is something that is happening in medicine, by the way, for doctors who are using AI as an assistant. This is this is really interesting. So, and this is dermatology was a paper in nature, you know, skin lesions. There's several 2000 skin lesions. Some of them are, you know, cancerous and others are benign.
And so, in any case, they tested the expert doctors, and then they tested an AI, and they were both doing about 90%. However, if you let the doctor use the AI, it boosts the doctor to 98%. 98% accuracy. Yes. And what's going on there? It's very interesting. So it turns out that
Although they got the same 90%, they had different expertise that the AI had access to more data, and so it could look at the lesions that were rare that the doctor may never have seen, okay? But the doctor has more in-depth knowledge of the most common ones that he's seen over and over again, and those are subtleties and so forth. But so, putting them together, it makes so much sense that they're going to improve if they work together.
And I think that now you're saying is that using AI as a tool for discovery with the expert who's interpreting and looking at the arguments, the statistical arguments, and also looking at the paper maybe in a new way,
Maybe that's the future of science. Maybe that's what's going to happen. Everybody is worried about, oh, AI is going to replace us. It's going to be much better than we are everything, and humans are obsolete. Nothing can be further from the case. Our strengths and weaknesses are different, and by working together, it's going to strengthen both
You know, what we do and what AI does. And it's going to be partnership. It's not going to be adversarial. It's going to be a partnership. Would you say that's the case for things like understanding or discovering treatments for neurologic illness, for avoiding large scale catastrophes? Like can it predict
macro movements. Let me give an example. Here in Los Angeles, there's occasionally an accident on the freeway. You have a lot of cameras over freeways nowadays. You have cameras in cars. You can imagine all of the data being sent in in real time, and you could probably predict accidents.
pretty easily. I mean, these are just moving objects, right? At a specific rate who's driving haphazardly, but you could also potentially signal take over of the brakes or the steering wheel of a car and prevent accidents. I mean, certain cars already do that, but could you essentially eliminate? Well, let's do something even more important. Let's eliminate traffic. I don't know if you can do that, but because that's a funnel problem, but
Could you predict physical events in the world into the future? Okay, this has already been done, not for traffic, but for hurricanes. So, as you know, the weather is extremely difficult to predict.
And except here in California where it's always going to be sunny here. But now what they've done is to feed a lot of previous data from previous hurricanes and also simulations of hurricanes. You can simulate them in a supercomputer. It takes days and weeks. So it's not very useful for actually accurately predicting where it's going to hit Florida. But what they did was after training up the AI,
on all of this data, it was able to predict with much better accuracy exactly where in Florida, it's gonna make landfall. And it does that on your laptop in 10 minutes. Incredible. So something just clicked for me, and it's probably obvious to you and most people, but I think this is true. I think what I'm about to say is true. At the beginning of our conversation, we were talking about
the acquisition of knowledge versus the implementation of knowledge. Just learning facts versus learning how to implement those facts in the form of physical action or cognitive action. Math problem is cognitive action, physical action. AI can do both knowledge acquisition. It can learn facts, long lists of facts and combinations of facts. But presumably, it can also run a lot of problem sets and solve a lot of problem sets.
I don't think, except with some crude still to me, examples of robotics, that it's very good at action yet, but it will probably get there at some point. Robots are getting better, but they're not doing what we're doing yet. But it seems to me that
as long as they can acquire knowledge and then solve different problem sets, different iterations of combinations of knowledge that basically they are in a position to take any data about prior events or current events and make pretty darn good predictions about the future and run those back to us quickly enough.
and to themselves quickly enough that they could play out the different iterations. And so I'm thinking, you know, one of the problems that seems to have really vexed neuroscientists and the field of medicine and the general public has been like the increase in the, at least diagnosis of autism. I've heard so many different hypotheses over the years. I think we're still pretty much in the fog on this one.
Could AI start to come up with new and potential solutions and treatments if they're necessary, but maybe get to the heart of this problem? It might, and it depends on the data you have. It depends on the complexity of the disease.
But it will happen. In other words, we will use those tools at the best we can, because obviously, if you could make any progress at all and jump into the future, wow, that would save lives. That would help so many people out there. I really think the promise here is so great that even though there are flaws and there are regulatory problems, we really, really have to really push
And we have to do that in a way that is going to help people in terms of making their jobs better and helping them solve problems that otherwise they would have had difficulty with and so forth. And it's beginning to happen, but these are early days. So we're at a stage right now
with AI that is similar to what happened after the first flight of the Wright brothers. In other words, the achievement that the Wright brothers made was to get off the ground 10 feet and to power forward with a human being 100 feet. That was it. That was the first flight.
And it took an enormous amount of improvements. The most difficult thing that had to be solved was control. How do you control it? How do you make it go in the direction you want it to go? And shades of what's happening now in AI is that we are off the ground. We were not going very far yet, but who knows where it will take us into the future.
Let's talk about Parkinson's disease, a depletion of dopamine neurons that leads to difficulty in smooth movement generation and also some cognitive and mood-based dysfunction. Tell us about your work on Parkinson's and what did you learn? As you point out, Parkinson's is first a degenerative disease. It's very interesting because
The dopamine cells are a particular part of the brain stem, and they are the ones that are responsible for procedural learning. I told you before about temporal difference. It's dopamine cells. It's a very powerful way for the global signal. It's called a neuromodulator because it modulates all the other signals taking place throughout the cortex.
And also, it's very important for learning sequences of actions that produce survival for survival. But the problem is that with certain environmental insults, especially toxins like pesticides,
Those neurons are very vulnerable, and when they die, you get all of the symptoms that you just described. The people who have lost those cells actually before the treatment, you know, L-dopa, which is a dopamine precursor, they actually became comatose, right? They didn't move. They were still alive, but they just didn't move at all.
You know, it locked in. It's called tragic, tragic. So when the first trials of El Dopa were given to them, it was magical because suddenly they started talking.
So, I mean, this is amazing, amazing. I'm curious, when they started talking again, did they report that their brain state during the locked in phase was slow velocity? Was it sort of like a dreamlike state or they felt like they were in a nap? Or were they in there like screaming to get out? Because their physical velocity obviously was zero. They're locked in after all. And I've long wondered when coming back from a run or
from waking up from a great night's sleep when I shift into my waking state, whether or not physical velocity and cognitive velocity are linked. Okay, that's a wonderful observation or a question, you know the answer. Okay, here's something that is really amazing. It was discovered, interestingly, when they tend to move slowly, as you said, but to them cognitively, they think they're moving fast.
Now, it's not because they can't move fast because you can say, well, can you move faster? Sure. And they move normal. But to them, they think they're moving at super velocity. So it's a set point issue. So it's a set point issue. Yes, it's all about set points. That's what's really going on. And the set point gets further and further down. Without moving at all, they think they're moving. I mean, this is what's going on. By the way, you can ask them, what was it like? We were talking to you and you didn't respond.
i didn't feel like it uh... to bring the fabulous in answer they have well that they that they get populated it because they didn't have enough energy they couldn't initiate they couldn't initiate actions that's one of the things that they have trouble with it with uh... movements in starting movement as you can tell i'm fascinated by this notion of cognitive velocity and again there may be a better or more accurate or official uh... uh... language for for it but
I feel like it encompasses so much of what we try to do when we learn. And the fact that during sleep, you have these very vivid dreams during rapid eye movement sleep. So cognitive velocity is very fast. Time perception is different than in slow wave sleep dreams. And I really think there's something to it as a at least one metric that relates to brain state. I've long thought that we know so much more about brain states during sleep than we do about wakeful brain states.
that we talk about focus, motivated, flow. I mean, these are not scientific terms. I'm not being disparaging of them. They're pretty much all we've got until we come up with something better. But we're biologists and neuroscientists and computational neuroscientists in your case. And we're like trying to figure out what brain state are we in right now. Our cognitive velocity is a certain value. But I think the more that people think about this,
You know I'll venture to say that the more that they think a little bit about their cognitive velocity at different times of day Right start to notice that there's a tends to be a few times of day for me it tends to be
early to late mid-morning, and then again in the evening after a little bit of trough and energy, that boy, that hour and a half each, that's the time to get real work done. I can mentally sprint far at those times. But there are other times of day when I don't care how much caffeine I drink, unless it's a stressful event that I need to meet the demands of that stress,
I just can't get to that faster pace while I'm also engaging. You can read faster, you can listen, but you're not using the information, you're not storing the information. That's right. What times a day for you are... I get most done in the morning and then you write later after dinner.
is also different though. I think in the morning, I'm better at creative stuff. And then I think that in the evening, I'm better at actually just cranking it out. Interesting. Given the relationship between body temperature and circadian rhythm, I would like to run an experiment that relates core body temperature to cognitive velocity. I've actually noticed
This is something that is just purely subjective, but the temperature at the salt inside the building is kept 75. It's like, you know, it's rock solid. But in the afternoon, I feel a little chilly.
It's probably my internal body temperature. Yeah, it's probably going down. And that may correspond to the loss of energy, the amount of the ability for the brain and everything else. By the way, you know, this is Q10, this is a jargon, every single enzyme in your every cell.
can go at different rates depending on the temperature. So the body temperature is doing this and all the cells are doing this too. It's an explanation. I'm not sure if it's the right one. Craig Heller, my colleague at Stanford in the biology department,
has beautifully described how the enzymatic control over pyruvate, I believe it is, controls muscular failure, that local muscular failure, you know, when people are, like, kind of move some resistance, has everything to do with the temperature, the local temperature.
that shuts down certain enzymatic processes that don't allow the muscles to contract the same way. He knows the details and he covered them on this podcast and I'm forgetting the details. He starts to go, wow, these enzymes are so beautifully controlled by temperature. And of course, his laboratory is focused on ways to bypass those temperature or to change temperature locally in order to bypass those limitations and to have shown them again and again. It's just incredible.
Yeah, I hear we're speculating about what it would mean for cognitive velocity, but I think it's such a different world to think about the underlying biology as opposed to just thinking about a drug. You increase dopamine and norepinephrine and epinephrine, the so-called catecholamines, and you're going to increase energy focus and alertness, but you're going to pay the price. You're going to have a trough in energy focus and alertness that's proportional to how much greater it was when you took the drug.
Boy, amphetamines are a good example. Boy, you're going a mile a minute.
when you're taking the drug, of course, you know, it's a stand. That's your impression. And the reality is you don't actually accomplish that much more. Have any LLMs, so AI been used to answer this really pressing question of what is going to be the consequence on cognition for these young brains that have been weaned while taking Ritalin, Adderall, Vivance, and other stimulants?
Because we have millions of kids that have been raised this time. Unlily done this experiment on our whole cadre, a whole generation. And I really would like to know the answer. I wonder if anybody's studying it. That's really a great question. Because we gave them speed effectively, the drug that causes the brain to be activated. But by the way, there's the consequence is that when it wears off,
have no energy, right? You just completely spent. Yeah, that's it. That's the pit. That's the pit. And so, but that's why you take more of it. You see, that's the problem is it's a spiral. I love how today you're making it so very clear how computation, how math and computers and AI now,
are really shaping the way that we think about these biological problems, which are also psychological problems, which are also daily challenges. I also love that we touched on mitochondria and how to replenish mitochondria. I want to make sure that we talk about a couple of things that I know are in the back of people's minds, no pun intended here, which are consciousness and free will.
Normally, I don't like to talk about these things, not because they're sensitive, but because I find the discussions around them typically to be more philosophical than neurobiological, and they tend to be pretty circular. And so you get people like Kevin Mitchell, who I think he has a book about free will, he believes in free will. You've got people like Robert Sapolsky wrote the book determined, he doesn't believe him free will.
How do you feel about free will? And is it even a discussion that we should be having? Well, if you go back 500 years, you know, the Middle Ages, the concept didn't exist, or at least not in the way we use it. Because everybody, it was the way that humans felt about the
you know, the world and how it worked and its impact on them was that it's all fate. They had this concept of fate, which is that there's nothing you can do that something is going to happen to you because of what's going on in the gods above or whatever it is, right? You attribute it to the physical forces around you that caused it.
not to your own free will, not to something that caused you to this to happen to you, right? So I think that these words, by the way that we use, free will, consciousness, intelligence, understanding, they're weasel words because you can't pin them down. There is no definition of consciousness that everybody agrees on. It's tough to solve a problem
scientific problem if you don't have a definition that you can agree on. And, you know, there's this big controversy about whether these large language models understand language or not, right? The way we do.
And what it really is revealing is we don't understand what understanding is. Literally, we don't have a really good argument or measure that you could measure someone's understanding and then apply it to the GDP and see whether it's the same. It probably isn't exactly the same, but maybe there's some continuum here we're talking about, right?
The way I look at it, it says, if an alien suddenly landed on Earth and started talking to us in English, right? And the only thing we could be sure of it was that it's not human. I met some people that I wondered about.
their terrestrial origins. Okay. Well, okay. Now that there's a big diversity amongst humans too. Yeah. Yeah. Yeah. Yeah. Yeah. Certain colleagues of ours at UCSD years ago, one in particular in the physics department, who I absolutely adore as a human being, just had such an unusual pattern of speech.
of behavior, totally appropriate behavior, but just unusual. In the middle of a faculty meeting, we just kind of turn to me and start talking while the other person was presenting. And I was like, maybe not now. And he would say, oh, OK.
But in any other domain, you'd say he was very socially adept. And so, you know, there's certain people that just kind of discard with convention. And you kind of want to like, is he an alien? It's kind of cool, in a cool way. Like, you know, he's one of my, again, a friend and somebody I really delight in. It's true, it's true. You know, no, no, not everybody has adopted the same social conventions. You know, it could be a touch of autism.
I mean, yeah, that's a problem that I mean in other words, they're very high functioning autistic people out there He's brilliant and often they are you know It's there are high people who are brilliant that with autism, but but you know could you build an LLM that was more
on one end of the spectrum versus the other to see what kind of information they forage for. I reviewed a paper. It seemingly would be a really important thing to do. It's been done. There was a paper that I reviewed where they took the LM and they fine-tuned it with different data from people with different disorders, the autism and so forth. And sociopaths
You know. Not scary. But you want to know the answer? No, no. And they got these LLMs to behave just like those people who have these disorders. You can get them to behave that way, yes. Could you do political leaning in values?
I haven't seen that, but it's pretty clear that to me, at least, that if you can do sociopathy, you can probably do any political belief. But you could also view all this as... You could take benevolent tracks. You could also say hyper-creative
sensitive to emotional tone of voices and find out what kind of information that person brings. Excuse me, that LLM brings back versus somebody who is very oriented towards just the content of people's words as opposed to what, you know, because among people you find this. You know, if you've ever left a party with a significant other and sometimes someone will say, I've had this experience with like, did you see that interaction between someone? Like, no, what are you talking about? Like, did you hear that? I'm like, no.