Dr. Erich Jarvis: The Neuroscience of Speech, Language & Music
en
August 29, 2022
TLDR: Rockefeller University Professor Dr. Erich Jarvis discusses vocal communication, language evolution, connections between language and movement, and how social media affects cognition with his research spanning comparative genomics of speech and language across species.

In this engaging episode of the Huberman Lab podcast with Dr. Erich Jarvis, a leading expert in the neurogenetics of language at Rockefeller University, we explore the intricate connections between speech, language, and music. Below is a comprehensive summary of the key insights discussed throughout the episode.
Defining Speech and Language
- Speech vs. Language: Dr. Jarvis clarifies the distinction between speech (the physical articulation of sounds) and language (the system of communication). He highlights that there isn't a strict dichotomy, as both functionalities are closely intertwined in the brain.
- Animal Communication: Various species demonstrate different forms of communication, with some, like songbirds, possessing the ability to imitate sounds similar to human speech.
Evolution of Vocal Communication
- Innate Learning: Dr. Jarvis discusses how certain animals, including humans and songbirds, are genetically predisposed to learn vocalizations, a skill that has evolved from their ancestors.
- Critical Periods: A significant topic includes the critical periods during which language acquisition is most effective in children but becomes increasingly challenging with age.
The Interconnection of Language, Music, and Movement
- Music and Dance: Surprisingly, the discussion delves into music as a precursor to language. Dr. Jarvis posits that the evolution of song could have paved the way for more complex verbal communication, indicating a shared neural basis between singing and speech.
- Motor Theory of Vocal Learning: This theory suggests that the neural pathways for speech are intricately linked to those controlling body movement, including dance, emphasizing the holistic nature of communication.
Learning Languages
- Bilingualism and Neural Plasticity: The episode emphasizes how adults can successfully learn languages but often find it more challenging than children due to decreased neural plasticity that accompanies age.
- Cultural Hybridization: When different cultural languages converge, children often create hybrid languages, showcasing an innate ability to learn phonemes and structures from multiple languages simultaneously.
The Mechanisms of Communication
- Semantic vs. Affective Communication: Dr. Jarvis distinguishes between semantic (the meaning of words) and affective communication (emotion conveyed through tone and expression). He shares that both types utilize similar brain circuits.
- Facial Expressions: There is a deep-seated connection between facial expressions and speech patterns, reflecting emotional states and aiding in conveying meaning.
The Impact of Technology on Language Evolution
- Texting and Social Media: The growth of texting has influenced modern language, leading to shorthand and less formal structures of communication. This shift raises questions about the potential decline in speech proficiency as quick forms of communication become more predominant.
Stuttering and Speech Disorders
- Neurogenetics of Stuttering: Stuttering is explored from a neurobiological perspective. Insights are given on how speech disorders can be linked to genetic and developmental factors, with certain brain areas playing critical roles.
- Therapeutic Approaches: Discussion includes methods like rhythm and behavioral therapies that assist individuals dealing with stuttering.
Actionable Insights for Enhancing Language Skills
- Practice Through Movement: Dr. Jarvis asserts that physical activities, such as dancing, can enhance cognitive ability and speech. Engaging with music and dance keeps the neural circuits active, helping maintain cognitive functions.
- Reading and Writing: Encouraging reading diverse texts and maintaining manual writing skills rather than solely relying on typing can foster better thought processes in language use.
In summary, this episode with Dr. Erich Jarvis serves as a fascinating exploration of the interplay between speech, language, music, and movement, enriched with scientific insights into our evolutionary past and the implications for modern communication. Whether interested in the development of human communication, language learning, or the neuroscience behind it, this discussion provides valuable perspectives.
Was this summary helpful?
Welcome to the Huberman Lab Podcast where we discuss science and science-based tools for everyday life.
I'm Andrew Huberman, and I'm a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology at Stanford School of Medicine. Today, my guest is Dr. Eric Jarvis. Dr. Jarvis is a professor at the Rockefeller University in New York City, and his laboratory studies the neurobiology of vocal learning, language, speech disorders, and remarkably, the relationship between language, music, and movement, in particular, dance.
His work spans from genomics, so the very genes that make up our genome and the genomes of other species that speak and have language, such as songbirds and parrots, all the way up to neural circuits. That is the connections in the brain and body that govern our ability to learn and generate specific sounds and movements coordinated with those sounds, including hand movements.
and all the way up to cognition. That is our ability to think in specific ways based on what we are saying and the way that we comprehend what other people are saying, singing, and doing. As you'll soon see, I was immediately transfixed and absolutely enchanted by Dr. Jarvis' description of his work and the ways that it impacts all the various aspects of our lives.
For instance, I learned from Dr. Jarvis that as we read, we are generating very low level of motor activity in our throat. That is, we are speaking the words that we are reading at a level below the perception of sound or our own perception of those words. But if one were to put an amplifier to measure the firing of those muscles in our vocal cords, we'd find that as we're reading information, we are actually speaking that information.
And as I learn and you'll soon learn, there's a direct link between those species in the world that have song and movement, which many of us would associate with dance, and our ability to learn and generate complex language. So for people with speech disorders, like stutter or for people who are interested in multiple language learning, bilingual, trilingual, et cetera, and frankly, for anyone who is interested in how we communicate through words written or spoken,
I'm certain today's episode is going to be an especially interesting and important one for you. Dr. Jarvis' work is so pioneering that he has been awarded truly countless awards. I'm not going to take our time to list off all the various important awards that he's received, but I should point out that in addition to being a decorated professor at the Rockefeller University, he is also an investigator with the Howard Hughes Medical Institute, the so-called HHMI.
And for those of you that don't know, HHMI investigators are selected on an extremely competitive basis that they have to re-up, that is they have to recompete every five years. They actually receive a grade every five years that dictates whether or not they are no longer a Howard Hughes investigator or whether or not they can advance to another five years
of funding for their important research, and indeed Howard Hughes and Bessieres are selected, not just for the rigor of their work, but for their pioneering spirit and their ability to take on high risk, high benefit work, which is exactly the kind of work that Dr. Jarvis has been providing for decades now.
Again, I think today's episode is one of the more unique and special episodes that we've had on the Huberman Lab podcast. I single it out because it really spans from the basic to the applied and Dr. Jarvis' story is an especially unique one in terms of how he arrived at becoming a neurobiologist. So for those of you that are interested in personal journey and personal story, Dr. Jarvis' is truly a special and important one.
Before we begin, I'd like to emphasize that this podcast is separate from my teaching and research roles at Stanford. It is, however, part of my desired effort to bring zero cost and consumer information about science and science-related tools to the general public. In keeping with that theme, I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast. Our first sponsor is Element. Element is an electrolyte drink with everything you need and nothing you don't. That means plenty of salt, magnesium and potassium, the so-called electrolyte, and no sugar.
Salt, magnesium, and potassium are critical to the function of all the cells in your body, in particular, to the function of your nerve cells, also called neurons. In fact, in order for your neurons to function properly, all three electrolytes need to be present in the proper ratios. And we now know that even slight reductions in electrolyte concentrations or dehydration of the body can lead to deficits in cognitive and physical performance.
Element contains a science-backed electrolyte ratio of 1,000 milligrams of sodium, 200 milligrams of potassium, and 60 milligrams of magnesium. I typically drink element first thing in the morning when I wake up in order to hydrate my body and make sure I have enough electrolytes.
And while I do any kind of physical training and after physical training as well, especially if I've been sweating a lot, if you'd like to try element, you can go to drink element. That's LMT dot com slash Huberman to claim a free element sample pack with your purchase. Again, that's drink element LMT dot com slash Huberman. Today's episode is also brought to us by waking up.
Waking Up is a meditation app that includes hundreds of meditation programs, mindfulness trainings, yoga nidra sessions, and NSDR non-sleep depressed protocols. I started using the Waking Up app a few years ago because even though I've been doing regular meditation since my teens and I started doing yoga nidra about a decade ago,
My dad mentioned to me that he had found an app, turned out to be the waking up app, which could teach you meditations of different durations and that had a lot of different types of meditations to place the brain and body into different states and that he liked it very much. So I gave the waking up app a try and I too found it to be extremely useful because sometimes I only have a few minutes to meditate, other times I have longer to meditate and indeed I love the fact that I can explore different types of meditation
to bring about different levels of understanding, about consciousness, but also to place my brain and body into lots of different kinds of states, depending on which meditation I do. I also love that the waking up app has lots of different types of yoga nidra sessions. For those of you who don't know, yoga nidra is a process of lying very still, but keeping an active mind. It's very different than most meditations. And there's excellent scientific data to show that yoga nidra and something similar to it, called non-sleep deep rest, or NSDR,
can greatly restore levels of cognitive and physical energy, even with just a short 10-minute session. If you'd like to try the Waking Up app, you can go to wakingup.com slash Huberman and access a free 30-day trial. Again, that's wakingup.com slash Huberman to access a free 30-day trial. And now for my discussion with Dr. Eric Jarvis. Eric, so great to have you here. Thank you.
very interested in learning from you about speech and language. And even as I asked the question, I realized that a lot of people, including myself, probably don't fully appreciate the distinction between speech and language. Speech, I think, of as the motor patterns, the production of sound that has meaning, hopefully. And language, of course, comes in various languages and varieties of ways of communicating.
But in terms of the study of speech and language and thinking about how the brain organizes speech and language, what are the similarities? What are the differences? How should we think about speech and language?
Yeah, well, I'm glad you, you know, inviting me here and I'm also glad to get that first question, which I consider a provocative one. The reason why, I've been struggling, what is the difference with speech and language for many years? And realize why am I struggling? It's because their behavioral terms, let's call them psychologically psychology developed kind of terms, that don't actually align exactly with brain function.
All right. And the question is, there are distinction between speech and language. And when I look at the brain of work that other people have done, work we have done, also comparative with animal models, like those who can imitate sounds, like parrots and some birds, I start to see there really isn't such a sharp distinction. So to get at what I think is going on, let me tell you how some people think of it now, that there's a separate language module in the brain.
that has all the algorithms and computations that influence the speech pathway on how to produce sound and the auditory pathway on how to perceive and interpret it for speech or for sound that we call speech.
And it turns out, I don't think there is any good evidence for a separate language module. Instead, there is a speech production pathway that's controlling our larynx, controlling our jaw muscles, that has built within it all the complex algorithms for spoken language. And there's the auditory pathway that has built within it all the complex algorithms for understanding speech.
not separate from a language module. And this speech production pathway is specialized to humans and parrots and songbirds, whereas this auditory perception pathway is more ubiquitous amongst the animal kingdom. And this is why dogs can understand, sit, see, and they say, come here, ball, boy, get the ball and so forth. Dogs can understand several hundred human speech words. Great apes, you can teach them for several thousand, but they can't say a word.
fascinating. Because you've raised a number of animal species early on here, and because I have a basically an obsession with animals since the time I was very small, I have to ask, which animals have language
which animals have modes of communication that are sort of like language. I've heard whale songs. I don't know what they're saying. They sound very beautiful, but they could be insulting each other for all I know. Very well, maybe dolphins, birds. What do we understand about modes of communication that are
like language, but might not be what would classically be called language. Yes, right. So modes of communication that people would define as language, in a very narrow definition, they would say production of sound, so speech. But what about the hands, to gesturing with the hands? What about a bird who is doing aerial displays in the air communicating information through body language, right?
Well, I'm going to go back to the brain. So what I think is going on is for spoken language, we're using the speech pathway in all the complex algorithms there. Next to the brain regions that are controlling spoken language are the brain regions for gesturing with the hands.
And that hand parallel pathway has also complex algorithms that we can utilize. And some species are more advanced in these circuits, whether it's sound or gesturing with hands, and some are less advanced. Now, we humans and a few others are the most advanced for the speech sounds or the spoken language, but a non-human primate can produce gesturing in a more advanced form than they could produce sound.
I'm not sure I got that across clearly, just to say that humans are the most advanced at spoken language, but not necessarily as big a difference at gestural language compared to some of the species. Very clear and very interesting and immediately prompts the question, have there been brain imaging or other sorts of studies evaluating neural activity in the context of
you know, cultures and languages, at least that I associate with a lot of hand movement, like Italian versus, I don't know, maybe you could give us some examples of cultures where language is not associated with as much overt hand movement. Yes. So as you and I are talking here today and people who are listening but can't see us, we're actually gesturing with our hands as we talk without knowing it or doing it unconsciously. And if we were talking on a telephone,
I would have one hand here and I would be gesturing with the other hand without even you seeing me, right? And so why is that? Some have argued and I would agree with base upon what we've seen is that there is an evolutionary relationship between the brain pathways that control speech production and gesturing. And the brain regions I mentioned are directly adjacent to each other. And why is that? I think that the brain pathways that control speech evolved out of the brain pathways that control body movement.
All right, and that's when you talk about Italian, French, English, and so forth, each one of those languages come with a learned set of gestures that you can communicate with. Now, how is that related to other animals? Well, Coco Gorilla, who has raised with humans for 39 years or more, learned how to do gesture.
communication. Learn how to sign language, so to speak, right? But Coco couldn't produce those sounds. Coco could understand them as well.
by seeing somebody sign or hearing somebody produce speech, but Coco couldn't produce it with her voice. And so what's going on there is that a number of species, not all of them, a number of species have motor pathways in the brain where you can do learned gesturing, rudimentary language if you wanted to say with your lens, even if it's not as advanced as humans, but they don't have this extra brain pathway for the sound.
so they can't gesture with their voice in the way that they gesture with their hands. I see.
One thing that I've wondered about for a very long time is whether or not primitive emotions and primitive sounds are the early substrate of language. And whether or not there's a bridge that we can draw between those in terms of just the basic respiration systems associated with different extreme feelings. Here's the way I'm imagining this might work.
When I smell something delicious, I typically inhale more, and I might say, or something like that. Whereas if I smell something putrid, I typically turn away, I wince, and I will exhale, or turn away, not ingest those molecules or inhale those molecules.
I could imagine that these are the basic dark and light contrasts of the language system. And as I say that, I'm saying that from the orientation of a vision scientist who thinks of all visual images built up in a very basic way of a hierarchical model of the ability to see dark and light. So I could imagine this kind of primitive to more sophisticated pyramid of sound to language
Is this a crazy idea? Do we have any evidence this is the way it works? No, it's not a crazy idea. And in fact, you hit upon one of the key distinctions in the field of research that I had started out in, which is vocal learning research. So for vocal communication, you have most vertebrate species vocalized.
But most of them are producing innate sounds that they're born with producing. That is, babies crying, for example, or dogs barking. And only a few species have learned vocal communication, the ability to imitate sounds. And that is what makes spoken language special. When people think of what's special about language, it's the learned vocalizations. That is what's rare.
And so this distinction between innateness and learned is more of a bigger dichotomy when it comes to vocalizations than for other behaviors in the animal kingdom. And when you go in the brain, you see it there as well. And so all the things you talked about, the breathing, the grunting, and so forth,
A lot of that is handled by the brainstem circuits right around the level of your neck and below, like a reflex kind of thing. Or even some emotional aspects of your behavior in the hypothalamus and so forth. But for a learned behavior, learning how to speak, learning how to play the piano, teaching a dog to learn how to do tricks, is using the four brain circuits.
And what has happened is that there's a lot of forebrain circuits that are controlling learning how to move body parts in these species, but not for the vocalizations. But in humans and in parrots and some other species, somehow we acquired circuits where the forebrain has taken over the brainstem and now using that brainstem not only to produce the innate behaviors or vocal behaviors, but the learned ones as well. Do we have any sense of when
modern or sophisticated language evolved. Thinking back to the species that we evolved from, and even within homo sapiens, has there been an evolution of language? Has there been a devolution of language?
Yeah, I would say, and to be able to answer that question, it does come with the caveat that I think we humans over-rate ourselves when compared to other species. And so it makes even scientists go astray and trying to hypothesize when you especially don't find fossil evidence of language that easily out there in terms of what happened in the past.
Amongst the primates, which we humans belong to, we are the only ones that have this advanced vocal learning ability. Now, it was assumed that it was only homo sapiens. Then you can go back in time now based upon genomic data, not only of us living humans, but of the fossils that have been found for homo sapiens.
of Neanderthals, of Denisovan individuals, and discover that our human ancestors supposedly hybridize with these other hominid species. And it was assumed that these other hominid species don't learn how to imitate sounds. I don't know of any species today that's a vocal learner that can have children with a non-vocal learning species.
I don't say it. It doesn't mean it didn't exist. And when we look at the genetic data,
from these ancestral hominids that, you know, where we can look at genes that are involved in learned vocal communication, they have the same sequence as we humans do for genes that function in speech circuits. So I think Neanderthals had spoken language. I'm not gonna say it's as advanced as what it is in humans, I don't know. But I think it's been there for at least between 500,000 to a million years that our ancestors had this ability
and that we've been coming more and more advanced with it culturally and possibly genetically. But I think it's evolved sometime in the last 500,000 to a million years. Incredible.
Maybe we could talk a little bit more about the overlap between brain circuits that control language and speech in humans and other animals. I was weaned in the neuroscience era where birdsong and the ability of birds to learn their Tudor song was a
was and still is a prominent field, sub field of neuroscience. And then of course neuroimaging of humans, speaking and learning, et cetera. And this notion of a critical period, a time in which languages learned more easily than it is later in life.
And the names of the different brain areas were quite different. It one opens the textbooks. We hear vernikies and brokers for the humans. And you look at the birds of it. I remember, you know, HPC at Robust Arch Strait area X. That's right. Yeah, et cetera. But for most of our listeners that those names won't mean a whole lot. But in terms of homologies,
between areas in terms of function. What do we know? And how similar or different are the brain areas controlling a speech and language and say a songbird and a young human child?
Yeah, so going back to the 1950s, and even a little earlier, and Peter Mahler and others who got involved in neuro-ethology, the study of neurobiology of behavior in a natural way, right? They started to find that behaviorally, there are these species of birds, like some birds and parents, and now we also know hummingbirds, just three of them, out of the 40-something bird groups out there on the planet, orders, that they can imitate sounds like we do.
And so that was a similarity. In other words, they had this kind of behavior that's more similar to us than chimpanzees have with us, or than chickens have with them, right? They're close to relatives. And then they discovered even more similarities, these critical periods, that if you remove a child, you know, this unfortunately happens where a child is feral and is not raised with human and goes through their puberty phase of growth,
becomes hard for them to learn a language as an adult. So there's this critical period where you learn best. And even later on, when you're in regular society, it's hard to learn. Well, the same birds undergo these same thing. And then it was discovered that if they become deaf, we humans become deaf, our speech starts to deteriorate without any kind of therapy.
If a non-human primate or, you know, or let's say a chicken becomes deaf, their vocalizations don't deteriorate, very little at least. Well, this happens in the vocal learning birds. So there were all these behavioral parallels that came along with a package, and then people looked into the brain, Fernando Nautabon, my former PhD advisor, and began to discover the area X you talked about, the robust nucleus of the archopalian,
And these brain pathways were not found in the species who couldn't imitate, so there was a parallel here. And then, jumping many years later, I started to dig down into these brain circuits to discover that these brain circuits have parallel functions with the brain circuits for humans, even though they're by a different name like Broca's and Leringer motor cortex. And most recently, we discovered not only the actual circuitry and the connectivity are similar,
but the underlying genes that are expressed in these brain regions in a specialized way different from the rest of the brain are also similar between humans and somber and parrots. So all the way down to the genes and now we're finding the specific mutations.
are also similar, not always identical but similar, which indicates remarkable convergence for so-called complex behavior in species separated by 300 million years from a common ancestor. And not only that, we are discovering that mutations in these genes that cause speech deficits in humans, like in FOXP2, if you put those same mutations or similar type of deficits in these vocal learning birds, you get similar deficits.
So convergence of the behavior is associated with similar genetic disorders of the behavior. Incredible. I have to ask, do hummingbirds sing or do they hum? Hummingbirds hum with their wings and sing with their serics. In a coordinated way.
in a coordinated way. There's some species of hummingbirds that actually will, Doug Ashford showed this, that will flap their wings and create a slapping sound with their wings that's in unison with their song. And you would not know it, but it sounds like a particular syllable in their songs, even though it's their wings and their voice at the same time.
Hummingbirds are clapping to their song. Clapping, they're snapping their wings together in unison with a song to make it like, if I'm going, ba-da-da-da-da-da-da-da-da-da-da-da-da-da-da. You know, I banged on the table, except they make it almost sound like their voice with their wings.
incredible. And they got some of the smallest brains you would say, mind blown, right? Yes, yes. Incredible. Yes. Incredible. I love hummingbirds and I always feel like it's such a special thing to get in a moment to see one because they move around so fast and they flit away so fast in these ballistic trajectories that when you get to see one stationary for a moment or even just hovering there, you feel like you're extracting so much from their little
little microcosm of life, but now I realize they're playing music essentially. Right, exactly. And what's amazing about hummingbirds, and I'm going to say vocal learning species in general, is that for whatever reason, they seem to evolve multiple complex traits. This idea that evolving language, spoken language in particular, comes along with a set of specializations. Incredible.
When I was coming up in neuroscience, I learned that, I think it was the work of Peter Marlar that young birds, songbirds, learn their Tudor song and learn it quite well.
but that they could learn the song of another tutoring. In other words, they could learn a different, and for the listeners, I'm doing air quotes here, a different language, a different bird song, different than their own species. But never as well as they could learn their own natural genetically linked song, genetically linked, meaning that they would be like me being raised in a different culture.
that I would learn the other language, but not as well as I would have learned English. This is the idea. Is that true? That is true, yes. And that's what I learned growing up as well and talk to Peter Mahler himself about before he passed. He used to call it the innate predisposition to learn, all right? So, which would be kind of the equivalent in the linguistic community of universal grammar. There is something genetically influencing
our vocal communication on top of what we learn culturally. And so there's this balance between the genetic control of speech or a song in these birds and the learned cultural control. And so yes, if you were to take, I mean, in this case, we actually tried this at Rockefeller later on, take a zebra finch,
and raised it with a canary, it would sing a song that was sort of like a hybrid in between, we call it a caninch, right? And vice versa for the canary, because there's something different about their vocal musculature or the circuitry in the brain.
And with a zebra finch, even with a closely related species, if you would take a zebra finch, a young animal, and in one cage next to it, place its own species, adult male, right? And in the other cage, place a Bengalese finch next to it. It would preferably learn the song from its own species neighbor. But if you remove its neighbor, it would learn that Bengalese finch very well. Fantastic. So it has something to do with also the social bonding with your own species.
Incredible.
That raises a question that based on something I also heard, but I don't have any scientific peer-reviewed publication to point to, which is this idea of pigeon, not the bird, but this idea of when multiple cultures and languages converge in a given geographic area, that the children of all the different native languages will come up with their own language. I think this was in island culture, maybe in Hawaii, called pigeon, which is sort of a hybrid of the various languages that their parents speak at home.
and that they themselves speak, and that somehow pigeon, again, not the bird, but a language called pigeon for reasons I don't know,
harbors certain basic elements of all language. Is that true? Is that not true? I haven't studied enough myself in terms of pigeons specifically, but in terms of cultural evolution of language and hybridization between different cultures and so forth, even amongst birds with different dialects, and you bring them together. What is going on here is
Cultural evolution remarkably tracks genetic evolution. So if you bring people from two separate populations together that have been in their separate populations evolutionarily, at least, for hundreds of generations. So someone's speaking Chinese, someone speaking English.
and that child then's learning from both of them. Yes, that child's going to be able to pick up and merge phonemes and words together in a way that an adult wouldn't. Because why they're experiencing both languages at the same time during their critical period years in a way that adults would not be able to experience.
And so you get a hybrid. And the lowest common denominator is going to be what they share. And so the phonemes that they've retained in each of their languages is what's going to be, I imagine, used the most. Interesting.
So we've got brain circuits in songbirds and in humans that in many ways are similar, perhaps not in their exact wiring, but in their basic contour of wiring, and genes that are expressed in both sets of neural circuits in very distinct species that are responsible for these phenomena we're calling speech and language.
What sorts of things are those genes controlling? I could imagine they were controlling the wiring of connections between brain areas, essentially a map of a circuit, basically like an engineer on a circuit for speech and language, nature designed for speech and language, but presumably other things too, like the ability to connect
motor patterns within the throat, of muscles within the throat, when the control of the tongue. I mean, what are these genes doing? You're pretty good. Yeah, you've made some very good guesses there that make sense. So, yes, one of the things that differ in the speech pathways of us and these psalm pathways of birds is some of the connections are fundamentally different than the surrounding circuits.
like a direct cortical connection from the areas that control vocalizations in the cortex, the motor neurons that control the larynx in humans or the serings in birds. And so we actually made a prediction that since some of these connections differ, we're going to find genes that control neuroconnectivity and that specialize in that function, that differ.
And that's exactly what we found. Genes that control what we call axon guidance and form instrument connections. And what was interesting, it was sort of in the opposite direction that we expected. That is, some of these genes, actually a number of them that control neural connectivity were turned off in the speech circuit.
All right. And it didn't make sense to us at first, and so we started to realize the function of these genes are to repel connections from forming, so repulsive molecules. And so when you turn them off, they allow certain connections to form that normally would have not formed. So by turning it off, you got to gain a function for speech, right? Other genes that surprised us were genes involved in calcium buffering, neural protection.
like a parvavamine or a heat shock protein. So when your brain gets hot, these proteins turn on. And we couldn't figure out for a long time, why is that the case? And then the idea popped to me one day and said, ah, when I heard the larynx is the fastest firing muscles in the body, all right? In order to vibrate sound and modulate sound in the way we do, you have to control, you have to move those muscles
three to four to five times faster than just regular walking or running.
And so when you stick electrodes in the brain areas that control learned vocalizations in these birds and I think in humans as well, those neurons are firing at a higher rate to control these muscles. And so what is that going to do? You're going to have lots of toxicity in those neurons unless you upregulate molecules that take out the extra load that is needed to control the larynx. And then finally, a third set of genes that are
specializing in these speed circuits are involved in neuroplasticity. Neuroplasticity meaning allowing the brain circuits to be more flexible so you can learn better. And why is that I think learning how to produce speech is a more complex learning ability than say learning how to walk or learning how to do tricks and jumps and so forth that dogs do.
Yeah, it's interesting as you say that because I realized that many aspects of speech are sort of reflexive. I'm not thinking about each word I'm gonna say. They just sort of roll out of my mouth. Hopefully with some forethought, we both know people that seem to speak, think less. Fewer synapses between their brain and their mouth than others, right? A lot of examples out there. And some people are very deliberate in their speech. But nonetheless, that much of speech has to be precise. And some of it less precise.
in terms of plasticity of speech and the ability to learn multiple languages, but even just one language, what's going on in the critical period, the so-called critical period? Why is it that, so my niece speaks Spanish, she's Guatemalan, speaks Spanish and English incredibly well. She's 14 years old. I've struggled with Spanish my whole life. My father's bilingual. My mother is not. I've tried to learn Spanish as an adult.
really challenging. I'm told that had I learned it when I was eight, I would be better off. Or it would be installed within me. So the first question is, is it easier to learn multiple languages without an accent early in life? And if so, why? And then the second question is, if one can already speak more than one language as a consequence of childhood learning, is it easier to acquire new languages later on?
So the answer to both of those questions is yes. But to explain this, I need to let you know actually the entire brain is undergoing a critical period development, not just the speech pathways. And so it's easier to learn how to play a piano. It's easier to learn how to ride a bike for the first time and so forth.
as a young child, then it is later in life. What I mean easier in terms of when you start from first principles of learning something. So the very first time if you're going to learn Chinese as a child versus the very first time you learn Chinese as an adult or learning to play piano as a child versus an adult.
But the speech pathways, or let's say speech behavior, I think has a stronger critical period changed to it than other circuits. And what's going on there in general?
Why do you need a critical period to make you more stable, to make you more stubborn, so to speak? The reason I believe is that the brain can only hold so much information. And if you are undergoing rapid learning to acquire new knowledge, you also have to
you know, dump stuff, put in memory or information in the trash, like in a computer. You only have so many gigabases of memory. And so therefore, plus also for survival, you don't want to keep forgetting things.
And so the brain is designed, I believe, to undergo this critical period and solidify the circuits with what you learned as a child and you use that for the rest of your life. And we humans stay even more plastic in our brain functions controlled by a gene called SRGAP2. We have an extra copy of it that leaves our speech circuit and other brain regions in a more immature state throughout life compared to other animals. So we're more immature.
We're still juvenile-like compared to other animals. I knew it. But we still go through the critical periods like they all do. And now, the question you asked about, if you learn more languages as a child, is it easier to learn as an adult? And that's a common finding out there in the literature. There are some that argue against it. But for those that support it, the idea there is you are born with a set of innate sounds you can produce of phonemes.
And you narrow that down because not all languages use all of them. And so you narrow down the ones you use to string the phonemes together in the words that you learn. And you maintain those phonemes as an adult. And here comes along another language that's using those phonemes or in different combinations you're not used to.
And therefore, it's like starting from first principles. But if you already have them in multiple languages that you're using, then it makes it easier to use them in another third or fourth language. I see. Incredible. So it's not like your brain has maintained greater plasticity. Your brain has maintained greater ability to produce different sounds that then allows you to learn another language faster. Got it.
Are the hand gestures associated with sounds or with meanings of words? I think the hand gestures associated with both the sounds and the meaning of when I say sound like if you are really angry, right? And you are making a loud screaming noise, right? You may make hand gestures that are look like you're going to beat the wall, right? Because you're making loud sounds and loud gestures.
All right? But if you want to explain something like, come over here, what I just do now to you for those who can't see me, I swung my hand towards you and swung it here to me. That has a meaning to it, to come here. So just like with the voice, the hand gestures are producing both qualities of sound.
And for people that speak multiple languages, especially those that learn those multiple languages early in development, do they switch their patterns of motor movements according to, let's say, going from Italian to Arabic, or from Arabic to French, in a way that matches the precision of language that they're speaking.
You know what? You just ask me a question. I don't know the answer to. I would imagine that would make sense because of switching in terms of sometimes people might call this code switching, even different dialects of the same language. Could you do that with your gestures? I imagine so, but I really don't know if that's true or not.
Well, I certainly don't know from my own experience because I only speak one language. I'd like to take a quick break and acknowledge one of our sponsors, Athletic Greens. Athletic Greens, now called AG1, is a vitamin mineral probiotic drink that covers all of your foundational nutritional needs.
I've been taking athletic greens since 2012, so I'm delighted that they're sponsoring the podcast. The reason I started taking athletic greens and the reason I still take athletic greens once or usually twice a day is that it gets to be the probiotics that I need for gut health. Our gut is very important. It's populated by gut microbiota that communicate with the brain, the immune system, and basically all the biological systems of our body to strongly impact our immediate and long-term health.
And those probiotics and athletic greens are optimal and vital for microbiota health. In addition, athletic greens contains a number of adaptogens, vitamins and minerals that make sure that all of my foundational nutritional needs are met. And it tastes great. If you'd like to try athletic greens, you can go to athleticgreens.com slash Huberman.
and they'll give you five free travel packs that make it really easy to mix up athletic greens while you're on the road, in the car, on the plane, et cetera. And they'll give you a year's supply of vitamin D3K2. Again, that's at letitgreens.com slash Huberman to get the five free travel packs and the year's supply of vitamin D3K2. To go a little bit into the abstract, but not too far, what about modes of speech and language that seem to have
a depth of emotionality and meaning, but for which it departs from structured language. Here's what I mean, poetry. I think of musicians, like there are some Bob Dylan songs that to me, I understand the individual words.
I like to think there's an emotion associated with it. At least I experienced some sort of emotion and I have a guess about what he was experiencing. But if I were to just read it linearly without the music and without him singing it or somebody singing it like him, it wouldn't hold any meaning. So in other words, words that seem to have meaning but not associated with language but somehow tap into an emotionality.
Yep, absolutely. So we call this different semantic communication, communication with meaning, and effective communication, communication that has more of an emotional feeling content to it, but not with the semantics. And the two can be mixed up, like with singing words that have meaning, but also have this effect of emotional, you just love the sound of the singer that you're hearing.
And initially, you know, psychologists, scientists in general thought that these were going to be controlled by different brain circuits. And it is the case, there are emotional brain centers and the hypothalamus in the cingulate cortex and so forth that do give tone to the sounds.
But I believe, you know, based upon imaging work and work we see in birds, when birds are communicating semantic information in their sounds, which is not too often, but it happens, versus effective communication saying, because I'm trying to attract the mate, my courtship song or defend my territory, it's the same brain circuits. It's the same speech-like or song circuits are being used in different ways.
A friend of mine who's also a therapist said to me, you know, it's possible to say I love you with intense hatred and to say I hate you with intense love. And reminding me that it's possible to hear both of those statements in either way. So I guess it's not just limited to song or poetry. It also, there's something about the intention
and the emotional context in which something's spoken that can heavily shape the way that we interpret what we hear. That's right. And I consider all of that actually meaning, even though I define that as people commonly do semantic and effective communication,
effective communication to say, I hate you, but men love, right? It does have emotional meaning to it. And so, one's more like an object kind of meaning or an abstract kind of meaning.
There's several other points here, I think it's important for those listening out there to hear, is that when I say also this effective and semantic communication being used by similar brain circuits, it also matters the side of the brain. In birds and in humans, there's left-right dominance for learned communication, learned sound communication. So the left in us humans is more dominant for speech.
But the right has a more balance for singing or processing musical sounds as opposed to processing speech. Both get used for both reasons. And so when people say your right brain is your artistic brain and your left brain is your thinking brain, this is what they're referring to. And so that's another distinction. The second thing that's useful to know is that all vocal learning species
use their learned sounds for this emotional, effective kind of communication. But only a few of them, like humans and some parrots and dolphins, use it for the semantic kind of communication calling speech.
That has led a number of people to hypothesize that the evolution of spoken language of speech evolved first for singing, for this more like emotional kind of made attraction like the Jennifer Lopez, the Ricky Martin kind of songs and so forth. And then later on, it became used for abstract communication like we're doing now. Interesting.
Well, that's a perfect segue for me to be able to ask you about your background and motor control, not only of the hands, but of the body. So you have a number of important distinctions to your name, but one of them is that you were a member of the Alvin Ailey dance.
school, school of dance. So you're an accomplished and quite able dancer, right? Tell us a little bit about your background in the world of dance and as how it informs your interest in neuroscience, excuse me, and perhaps even how it relates specifically to your work on speech and language.
Yes. Well, it's interesting. And this kind of history even goes before my time. So in my family, my mother and father's side, they both went to the high school of music and art here in New York City. And particularly in my mother's family, going back multiple generations, they were singers. And I even did my family genealogy and found out, not only we have some relationships to some well-known singers, distant relationships like Thelonious Monk,
But going back to the plantations in North Carolina and so forth, my ancestors were singers in the church for the towns and so forth. And this somehow got passed on multiple generations to my family. And I thought I was going to grow up and be a famous singer, right? And me and my brothers and sister formed the band when we were kids and so forth.
But it turned out that I didn't inherit the singing talents of some of my other family members, even though it was okay, but not like my brother, or not like my mother, or my aunts, and my cousin, Pudafee, who is now a talented Native American singer. That then influenced me to do other things. And I started competing in dance contests.
You know, actually just around the time of the Saturday Night Fever, and I was a teenager, and I started winning dance contests, and I thought, oh, I can dance. And I auditioned for the high school of performing arts, and I got in here in New York City, and got into ballet dance, and got in, right, and thought, if I learn ballet, I can learn everything else. It was that idea. If you learn something classical, it can teach you for everything else.
And I was, yeah, at Alvin Ailey Dance School, Joffrey Ballet Dance School. And at the end of my senior concert, I had this opportunity to audition for the Alvin Ailey Dance Company. And I had an opportunity to go to college. And I also fell in love with another passion that my father had, which was science.
And so I liked science in high school, and I found an overlap also between the arts and sciences. You know, both required creativity, hard work, discipline, you know, new discovery, both weren't boring to me. And the one decision I made at that senior dance concert was, you know, talking to the Alvin Ailey recruiter and thinking about it, I have to make a decision.
And I thought something my mother taught me, because she was grown up in the 1960s cultural revolution, do something that has a positive impact on society. And I thought that I can do that better as a dancer than a scientist. So now, jump. I get into college. Under graduate school, I major in molecular biology and mathematics. I decide I want to be a biologist. Got into graduate school when it's a study of the brain at the Rockefeller University. So I went from Hunter College to Rockefeller University.
And so now I got to the brain. And why did I choose the brain is because it controls dancing. But there was anybody studying dancing. And I wanted to study the brain, something that's really interesting and complex. And I thought, ah, language is what it does. You couldn't study that in mice. You couldn't study non-human primates. But these birds do this wonderful thing that Fernando Nautubon was studying at Rockefeller.
And so that's what got me into the birds. And then jumping now 15 years later, that's right, even after I'm into now having my own lab studying vocal learning in these birds as a model for language and humans, it turns out that Ani Patel and others have discovered that only vocal learning species can learn how to dance.
Is that right? That's right. Yes. So I've seen these, I'm just scrolling through the files here in my mind. I think about, every once in a while someone was, I love parents. Yes. Every once in a while someone will send me one of these little Instagram or Twitter videos of a parent doing what looks to me like dance. Typically it's a cockatoo. That's right. That's right. Even foot stomping to the sound. The famous one called Snowball out there, but there are many Snowballs out there.
They're all the dancing birds are named snowball. That's interesting tactic. So only animals with language dance. Yeah. Vocal learning in particular, the ability to imitate sounds. Yes. Incredible. Yes. And this now is bringing my life full circle.
And so when that was discovered in 2009, at that same time in my lab at Duke, we had discovered that vocal learning brain pathways in songbirds, as well as in humans and in parrots, like snowball, are embedded within circuits that control learning how to move. And that led us to a theory we call the brain pathway or motor theory of vocal learning origin.
where the brain pathways for vocal learning and speech evolve by a whole duplication of the surrounding motor circuits involving learning how to move.
Now, how does that explain dance, right? Well, when snowball, the cockatoos are dancing, they're using the brain regions around their speech-like circuits to do this dancing behavior. And so what's going on there? What we hypothesize and now like to test is that when speech evolved in humans and the equivalent behavior in parents and songbirds,
It required a very tight integration in the brain regions that can hear sound with the brain regions that control your muscles from moving your larynx and tongue and so forth for producing sound.
In that tight auditory motor integration, we argue then contaminated the surrounding brain regions. And that contamination of the surrounding brain regions now allows us humans, in particular, in parrots, to coordinate our muscle movements of the rest of the body with sound in the same way we do for speech sounds. So we're speaking with our bodies when we dance.
Incredible. And I have to say that as poor as I am at speaking multiple languages, I'm even worse at dancing. So... But I guarantee you're better than a monkey. But not snowball the cockatoo. Maybe not snowball. On YouTube, we have a video where there's some scientists dancing with snowball, and you'll see snowballs doing better than some of the scientists. Okay, well, as long as I'm not the worst of all scientists at dancing, there's always neuroplasticity. May it save me someday.
You said something incredible that I completely believe, even though I have minimum to, let's just say minimum dancing ability. I can get by at a party or wedding without complete embarrassment, but I don't have any structured training. So the body clearly can communicate with movement. As a trained dancer and knowing other trained dancers, I always think of
dance and bodily movement and communication through bodily movement as a form of wordlessness, like a state of wordlessness. In fact, the few times when I think that maybe I'm actually dancing modestly well for the context that I'm in, where I see other people dancing and they seem to just be very much in the movement, it's almost like a state of non-language, non-spoken language.
Yet, what you're telling me is that there's a direct bridge at some level between the movement of the body and language. Is there a language of the body that is distinct from the language of speech?
If so, or if not, how do those map onto one another? What does that Venn diagram look like? Yeah, yeah. So let me define first dance in this context of vocal learning species. This is the kind of dancing that we are specialized in doing, and the vocal learning species are specialized in doing, is synchronizing body movements of muscles to the rhythmic beat of music.
And for some reason, we like doing it. We like synchronizing to sound and doing it together as a group of people. And that kind of communication amongst ourselves is more like the effective kind of communication I mentioned earlier, unlike the semantic kind.
So we humans are using our voices more for the semantic abstract communication, but we're using learned dance for the effective emotional bonding kind of communication. It doesn't mean we can't communicate semantic information in dance and we do it.
But it's not as popular, like a ballet in the Nutcracker. It is popular where they are communicating. The Arabian guy comes out, which I was the Arabian guy in the ballet Nutcracker. That's what I remember. Yeah, for the Westchester Ballet Company when I was a teenager.
We're trying to communicate meaning in our ballet dance and it can go on with a whole story and so forth. But people don't interpret that as clearly as speech. They're seeing the ballet with semantic communication with a lot of emotional content.
Whereas you go out to a club, you're not communicating, okay, how are you feeling today? Tell me about your day and so forth. You're trying to synchronize with other people in an effective way. And I think that's because the dance brain circuit inherited the more ancient part of the speech circuit, which was for singing.
I always had the feeling that with certain forms of music, in particular opera, but any kind of music where there are some long notes sung, that at some level there was a literal resonance created between the singer and the listener. Or I think of the deep voice of a Johnny Cash, or where at some level you can almost feel the voice in your own body.
In theory, that could be the vibration of the firing of the phrenic nerve controlling the diaphragm for all I know. Is there any evidence that there's a coordination between performer and audience at the level of mind and body?
I'm going to say possibly yes. And the reason why is because I just came back from a conference on the neurobiology of dance. Clearly, I'm going to the wrong meeting. The college, you know, vision science might be so boring. Yes. Well, one of my colleagues comes to Fitch and Jonathan Fritz. They organized a particular section on this conference in Virginia.
And this is the first time I was in the room with so many neuroscientists studying the neurobiology of dance. It's a new field now in the last five years.
And there was one lab where they were putting EG electrodes on the dancers, on two different dancers partnering with each other, as well as the audience seeing the dance. And some argued, OK, if you're listening to the music as well, how you respond. Because you're asking a question about music, and I'm giving you an answer about dance.
And what they found is that the dancers when they resonated with each other during the dance, or the audience listening to the dancers and the music, there are some resonance going on there that they've scored as higher resonance. Their brain activity with these wireless EEG signals are showing something different.
And so that's why I say possibly it. It needs more rigorous study. And this is some stuff they publish, but it's not primetime yet, but they're trying to figure this out.
So at least if I can't dance well, maybe I can hear and feel what it is to dance in a certain way. That's right. And this will be, some people will think that they, even songs that they hear, and they can almost sing to themselves in their own head. And they know what they want it to sound like. And you know when it really sounds good, what it sounds like, but they can't get their voice to do it.
For those listening, I'm raising my hand. No musical ability. Others in my household have tremendous musical ability with instruments and with voice, but not me. And so this is one of my selfish goals of trying to find the genetics of why can some people sing really well and some not? Is there some genetic predisposition to that? And then can I modify my own muscles or brain circuits to sing better?
You're still after the thing. I guess this is what happens when siblings are very in proficiency is that competitiveness among brothers and sisters never goes away. I've been trying to be as good as my brother Mark and Victor for the hope my entire life. Watch out, Mark and Victor. He's coming for you with neuroscience to back him.
Earlier you said that you discovered that you could dance. That caught my ear. It sounds like you didn't actually have to, I'm not suggesting you didn't work hard at it, but that at the moment where you discovered it, it just sort of was a skill that you had that up until that point, you didn't target a life in the world of dance. But the fact that you quote unquote discovered that you could dance really well and then went into this incredible school of dance and did well,
tells me that perhaps there is an ability that was built up in childhood, and or that perhaps we do all have different genetic leanings for different motor functions. Yeah. Well, for me, both explanations could be possible for the first. I grew up in its family listening to Motown songs, dancing at parties and so forth, family parties, an African-American family basically.
And so I grew up dancing from a young child, but this discovery, maybe dancing even more so in terms of a talent,
It could, the genetic component, if it really exists, I don't know. You know, with my 23andMe results, you know, it says I have the genetic substitutions that are associated with, you know, high intensity athletes and fast push muscles. And who knows, maybe that could have something to do with me being able to synchronize my body to rhythmic sounds, maybe.
maybe better than some others. It turns out that my genetics also show that I have a genetic substitute that makes it hard for me to sing on pitch. And so that does correlate with my, even though I can sing on this pitch especially if I hear a piano or kind of playing it, but maybe that's why my siblings who didn't have that genetic predisposition in his 23andMe results could go along with the genetic component as well.
I'm imagining family gatherings with 23andMeData and intense arguments about it, innate and learnability. Yes. Fun. Love to be an attendant. I'm not inviting myself to your Thanksgiving dinner, by the way. But I suppose I am. You're welcome, Tim. Thank you. I'll bring my 23andMeData.
I'd love to chat a moment about facial expression because that's a form of motor pattern that, you know, I think for most people out there just think about smiling and frowning, but there are, of course, you know, thousands if not millions of micro expressions and things of that sort, many of which are subconscious. And we're all familiar with the fact that
when what somebody says doesn't match some specific feature of their facial expression that it can call up, you know, that mismatch can cue our attention, especially among people that know each other very well. Yeah. Like you, somebody will say, well, you said that, but you, your right eye, twitch to the, you know, a little bit in a way that tells me that you didn't really mean that this, these kinds of things. Or
In the opposite example, when the motionality and the content of our speech is matched to a facial expression, there's something that's just so wonderful about that because it seems like everything's aligned. So how does the motor circuitry that controls facial expression map onto the brain circuits that control language speech and even bodily and hand movements?
You ask a great question because we both know some colleagues like Minric Freivold at Rockefeller University who studied facial expression and the neurology behind it. And now we both share some students that were co-mentoring and talk about this same question that you brought up. And what I'm learning a lot is that non-human primates have a lot of diversity in their facial expression like we humans do. And what we know about the neurobiology
of brain regions controlling those muscles of the face is that these non-human primates and some other species that don't learn how to imitate vocalizations, they have strong connections from the cortical regions to the motor neurons that control facial expressions. But absent connections or weak connections to the motor neurons that control the voice.
So I think our diverse facial expression, even though it's more diverse in these nonhuman primates, there was already a preexisting diversity of communication, whether it's intentional or unconscious through facial expression in our ancestors.
And on top of that, we humans now add the voice along with those facial expressions. I see. And in terms of language learning, when we're kids, children, fortunately, are not told to fake their expressions or to smile when they say, I'm happy.
So at some point, everybody learns for better or for worse how to untangle these different components of hand movement, body posture, speech, and facial expression. But in their best form, I would say.
assuming that the best form is always, I guess there are instances where, for safety reasons, one might need to feign some of these aspects of language. But in most cases, when those are aligned, it seems like that could reflect that all the different circuities are operating in parallel, but that the ability to misalign these is also a powerful aspect to our maturation. I didn't think of theater, for instance, where deliberate
disentangling of these areas is important, but also we know when an actor, when it feels real. And when it looks like, when bad acting is oftentimes when the facial expression or body posture just doesn't quite match what we're hearing. So are these skills that people that learn and acquire according to adaptability and profession or do you think that all children and all adults eventually learn how to couple and uncouple these circuits a little bit?
Yeah, I think it's this similar argument I mentioned earlier about the innate and the learned for the vocalizations. And by the way, when I say we humans have facial expressions associated with our vocalizations in a different way than primates, nonhuman primates, it's the learned vocalizations I'm talking about. So there is a
common view out there that facial expressions and non-human species, like non-human primates, or you can have them in birds too, are innate. And so they're reflexive and controlled. I don't believe that. I think there's some learned component to it. And I think we have more learning component to it as well.
But we also have an innate component. And so if you try to put your hands behind your back and hold your fist or even just not and try to speak and try to communicate, it's actually harder to do. You have to force yourself or put it by your side.
This comes naturally. Facial expressions comes naturally because there is an innate component. And yes, you have to learn how to dissociate the two, communicate something angry with your hands or with your face, but, you know, politely with your voice. It's very hard to separate at those two because there is that innate component that brings them together.
So it's like an email, too. You're emailing, and someone says something by email. Someone can interpret that angrily or gently. And it becomes ambiguous. The facial expressions get rid of that ambiguity. So glad you brought that out, because my next question was, and is about written language.
The first question I'll ask is, when you write, either type or write things out by hand, do you hear the content of what you want to write in your head? You just you personally. Yes, I do.
And I know that I do because I was trying to figure out a debate about this issue and trying to resolve the debate with my own self-experimentation on me. I ask that because a quite well-known colleague of ours, Carl Dicerath.
at Stanford, who's been on this podcast and is, you know, of optogenetics fame and psychiatry fame, et cetera. And I know him. Yeah. He sends his regards. He told me that his practice for writing and for thinking involves a quite painful process of forcing himself to sit completely still.
and think in complete sentences, to force thinking in complete sentences. And when he told me that, I decided to try this exercise. And it's quite difficult. First of all, it's difficult for the reason that you mentioned, which is that with many thoughts, I want to look around and I start to justiculate with my hands. So there it is again, the connection between language and hand movement, even if one isn't speaking.
And the other part is that I, that's challenging is I realized that while we write in complete sentences, most of the time, we'll talk about how that's changing now and texting, et cetera, that
we don't often think in complete sentences and specifically in simple declarative sentences that a lot of our thoughts would be if they were written out onto a page would look pretty much like passive language that a good copy editor or a good editor would say, oh, we need to cross this out, make this simple and declarative. So what I'm getting at here is what is the process of going from a thought to language to written word?
And I also wanted to touch on handwritten versus typed, but thought to language to written word. What's going on there? What do we know about the neural circuitry? And I was going to ask, why is it so hard? But now I want to ask, why is this even possible? It seems like a very challenging neural computational problem. Yeah.
Yeah, and coming from the linguistic world, even just the regular neurobiology world, going back to something I said before is about a separate language module in the brain. There was this thought or hypothesis that this language module has all these complex algorithms to them.
And they're signaling to the speech circuit, how to produce the sounds, the hand circuit, how to write them or gesture the visual pathway on how to interpret them from reading and the auditory pathway for listening.
I don't think that's the case, all right? And that this thinking where there's this internal speech going on. What I think is going on is to explain what you're asking is about that I'm going to take it from the perspective of reading something. You read something on a paper. The signal from the paper goes through your eyes. It goes to the back of your brain to your visual cortical regions eventually.
And then you now got to interpret that signal in your visual pathway of what you're reading. How are you going to do that in terms of speech? That visual signal then goes to your speech pathway in the motor cortex in front here and broke his area. And you silently speak what you read in your brain without moving your muscles.
But sometimes, actually, if you put electrodes, EEG, EMG electrodes on your laryngeal muscles, even on birds, you can do this. You'll see activity there while reading or trying to speak silently, even though no sound's coming out. And so, your speech pathway is now speaking what you're reading.
Now to finish it off, that signal is sent to your auditory pathway so you can hear what you're speaking in your own head.
That's incredible. And this is why it's complicated because you're using like three different pathways, the visual, the speaking motor one and the auditory to read. Oh, and then you got to write, right? Okay, here comes the fourth one. Now the hand area's next to your speech pathway is got to take that auditory signal or even the adjacent motor signals for speaking and translate it into a visual signal on paper.
So you're using at least four brain circuits, which includes the speech production and the speech perception pathways to write. Incredible. And finally explains to me why, so I was weaned teaching undergraduates, graduate students, and medical students, and I've observed that
when I'm teaching, I have to stop speaking if I'm going to write something on the board. I just have to stop all speaking completely. It turns out this is an advantage because it allows me to catch my voice. It allows me to slow down a bit, breathe and inhale some oxygen and so on because I tend to speak quickly if I'm not writing something out. So there's a break in the circuitry for me, or at least they are distinct enough that I have to stop and then write something.
Yes, that does imply competing brain circuits for your conscious attention. We have colleagues up at Columbia Med who are known at least in our circles for voice dictating their papers, not writing them out, but just speaking into a voice recorder. I've written papers that way. It doesn't feel quite
as natural for me as writing things out, but not because I can go quickly from thought to language to typing. I type reasonably fast. I can touch type now. I don't think I ever taught myself. I never took a touch type course. It just sort of happened now. I think my motor system seems to know where the keys are with enough accuracy that it works. This is remarkable to me that any of us can do this, but
When it comes to writing, what I've found is that if my rate of thought and my rate of writing are aligned nicely, things go well. However, if I'm thinking much faster than I can write, that's a problem. And certainly if I'm thinking more slowly than I want to write, that's also a problem. And the solution for me has been to write with a pen. I'm in love with these and I have no relationship to the company.
At least not now, although if they want to work with us, I love these Pilot V5-V7s, because not necessarily because of the ink or the feel, although I like that as well, but because of the rate that it allows me to write. They write very well slowly, and they write very well quickly. And so I have this theory.
supported only by my own ANIC data, no peer reviewed study, that writing by hand is fundamentally different than typing out information. Is there any evidence that this motor pathway for writing is better or somehow different than the motor pathway for typing?
Yeah, that's interesting. And I don't know of any studies. I have my own personal experience as well, but trying to put this into the context. If I had to design an experiment to test the hypothesis here, to explain your experience in mind, is that writing by hand, I would argue requires a different set of less skills with the fingers than typing.
So you have to coordinate your fingers more in opposite directions and so forth with typing, but also writing by hand requires more arm movement. And so therefore, I would argue that the difficulty there could be in the types of muscles and the fine motor control you need of those muscles along with speaking in your brain at the same time.
So basically I'm coarse, I'm a brute. And so it makes sense that a more primitive writing device would work. That's right, yes. But let me add to this in terms of my own personal experience, right? What I find is I can write something faster by hand for a short period of time compared to typing. And that is because I think I run out of the energy in my arm movements
faster than I run out of muscle energy in my finger movements. And I think it takes a longer time for us to write words without fingers because in terms of the speech. So I think you're writing, whether it's by hand or typing, into your speech. It only will align very well if you can type as fast as you can speak or write as fast as you can speak in your head.
I love it. So what you've done, if I understand correctly, is created a bridge between thought and writing, and that bridge is speech. That bridge is speech. That's right. That's right. When you're writing something out, you're speaking it to yourself. And if you're speaking faster than you can type, you got a problem.
Interesting. I do a number of podcast episodes that are not with guests, but solo episodes. And as listeners know, these are very long episodes, often two or more hours. And we joke around the podcast studio that I will get locked into a mode of speech where some of it is more elaborative and anecdotal. And then I'll punch out simple declarative sentences.
I find it very hard to switch from one module to the next. The thing that I have done in order to make that transition more fluid and prep for those podcast episodes is actually to read the lyrics of songs and to sing them in my head.
as a way of warming up my vocal cords, but luckily for those around me, when I do that, I'm not actually singing out loud. And so this, what you're telling me supports this idea that even when we are imagining
singing or writing in our mind, we are exercising our vocal chords. You're actually getting little low potentials of electrical currents reaching your muscles there, which also means you're exercising your speech brain circuits too, without actually going with the full volume activity in the muscles. Incredible.
And this idea of singing helps you as well, even with Parkinson's patients and so forth, when they want to say something, singing or listening to music helps them move better. And the idea there is that the brain circuits for singing, or let's say the function of the brain circuits for speech being used for singing first is the more ancestral trait. And that's why it's easier to do things with singing sometimes than it is with speaking. I love it.
Stutter is a particularly interesting case. And one that every once in a while, I'll get questions about this from our audience. Stutter is complicated in a number of ways. But culturally, in my understanding from these emails that I receive is that stutter can often cause people to hide and speak less because it can be embarrassing. And we're, we are often not patient with stutter. We also have the assumption that if somebody's stuttering,
that their thinking is slow, but it turns out there are many examples historically of people who could not speak well but who were brilliant thinkers. I don't know how well they could write, but they found other modes of communication. I realize that you're not a speech pathologist or therapist, but what is the current neurobiological understanding of stutter and what's being developed in terms of treatments for stutter?
Yeah, so we actually accidentally came across stuttering in songbirds. And we've published several papers on this to try to figure out the neurobiological basis. The first study we had was a brain area called the basal ganglio. The striatum part of the basal ganglio involved in coordinating movements, learning how to make movements. When it was damaged in a speech-like pathway in these birds,
What we found is that they started to stutter as the brain region recovered.
And unlike humans, they actually recovered after three or four months. And why is that the case? Because bird brains undergoes new neurogenesis in a way that human or mammal brains don't. And it was the new neurons that were coming in into the circuit, but not quite, you know, with the right proper activity was resulting in this stuttering in these birds.
And after it was repaired, not exactly the old song came back after the repair, but still it recovered a lot better. And it's now known, they call this neurogenic stuttering in humans, would damage to the basal ganglia, or some type of disruption to the basal ganglia at a young age, also causes stuttering in humans.
And even those who are born with stuttering, it's often the basal ganglia that's disrupted in some other brain circuit. And we think the speech part of the basal ganglia. Can adults who maintain a stutter from childhood repair that stutter?
They can repair it with therapy, with learning how to speak slower, learning how to tap out a rhythm. And yeah, I'm not a speech pathologist, but I started reading this literature and talking to others that colleagues who actually study studying.
So yes, there are ways to overcome the stuttering through behavioral therapy. And I think all of the tools out there have something to do with sensory motor integration. Controlling with you here with what you output in a thoughtful controlled way helps reduce the stuttering.
There are a couple examples from real life that I want to touch on, and one is somewhat facetious, but now I realize is a serious neurobiological issue, serious meaning I think interesting, which is that every once in a while,
I will have a conversation with somebody who says the last word of the sentence along with me. And it seems annoying in some instances, but I'm guessing this is just a breakthrough of the motor pattern that they're hearing what I'm saying very well. So I'm going to interpret this kindly and think they're hearing what I'm saying.
They're literally hearing it in their mind, and they're getting that low-level electrical activity to their throat, and they're just joining me in the enunciation of what I'm saying, probably without realizing it. Can we assume that that might be the case? Well, I wouldn't be surprised. The motor theory of speech perception, where this idea originally came, what you hear is going through your speech circuit, and then also activating those muscles slightly.
So, yes. So one might argue, okay, is that speech circuit now interpreting what that person is speaking, now you're listening to me, and is going to finish it off because it's already going through their brain, and they can predict it. That would be one theory. I don't think they've heard it out there as no, but that's one. The other is a synchronizing turn-taking in the conversation.
where you're acknowledging that we understand each other by finishing off what I say. And it's almost like a social bonding kind of thing. The other could be, I want the person to shut out so I can speak as well and take that turn. And each pair of people have a rhythm to their conversation. And if you have somebody who's over talkative versus under talkative or vice versa, that rhythm can be lost in them finishing ideas and going back and forth.
But I think having something to do with turn taking as well makes a lot of sense. I have a colleague at Stanford who says that interruption is a sign of interest. I'm not sure that everyone agrees. I think it's highly contextual. But there is this form of a verbal nod, I was saying, or things of that sort. And there are many of these. And I'm often told by my audience that I'd interrupt my guests and things of that sort. Oftentimes I'll just get caught in the natural flow of the conversation.
But I think we've had pretty good turn taking here, I hope. That's so far so good. I'm glad you feel that way, because especially in the context of a discussion about language, this seems important. Texting.
is a very, very interesting evolution of language. Because what you've told us is that we have a thought. It's translated into language. It might not be complete sentences, but texting, I have to imagine this is the first time in human evolution where we've written with our thumbs. So I don't, you know, it seems more primitive to me than typing with fingers around the hands, but hey, who might've judged the evolution of our species in one direction or the other? But the shorthand
grammatically, often grammatically deficient, incomplete sentence form of texting is an incredible thing to see. Early in relationships, romantic relationships, people will often evaluate the others' text and their ability to use proper grammar and spelling, et cetera. This often quickly degrades, and there's an acceptance that we're just trying to communicate through shorthand.
military-like shorthand, but with internally consistent between people, but there's no general consensus of what things mean. But you know, WTFs and like, and OMGs and all sorts of things.
I wonder sometimes whether or not we are getting less proficient at speech because we are not required to write and think in complete sentences. I'm not being judgmental here. I see this in my colleagues. I see this in myself. This is not a judgment of the younger generation. I also know that slang has existed
for decades, if not hundreds of years, but I also know that I don't speak the same way that I did when I was a teenager because I've suppressed a lot of that slang, not because it's inappropriate or offensive, although some of it was, frankly, but because it's out of context. So what do you think's happening to language? Are we getting better at speaking, worse at speaking, and what do you think the role of things like texting and tweeting and shorthand communication, hashtagging,
And what's that doing to the way that our brains work?
Yeah, I think that one, in terms of measuring your level of sophistication and intelligence, and you say OMG, I think that also could be a cultural thing, that you belong to the next generation. Or you're being cool, if you're an older person using OMG and other things that the younger generation would use. But really think about it clearly.
texting actually has allowed for more rapid communication amongst people. I think without the invention of the phone before then or texting back and forth, you had to wait days for a letter to show up. You couldn't call somebody on the phone and talk as well, so this rapid communication in terms of the rapid communication of writing in this case.
So, I think actually it's more like a use it or lose it kind of a thing with the brain. The more you use a particular brain region or circuit, the more enhance. It's like a muscle. The more you exercise it, the more healthier it is, the bigger it becomes and the more space it takes and the more you lose something else.
So I think texting is not decreasing the speech prowess or the intellectual prowess of speech. It's converting it and using it a lot in a different way.
In a way that may not be as rich in regular writing because you can only communicate so much nuance in short term writing. But whatever that, whatever is being done, you got people texting hours and hours and hours on the phone. So whatever your thumb circuit is going to get pretty big, actually.
I do wonder whether many people have lost their jobs based on tweets. The short latency between thought and action and distribution of one's thoughts is incredible. I'm not just talking about people who have
who apparently would have poor prefrontal, top-down control. This is geek speak, by the way, for people that lack impulse control, but high-level academics. I'm not going to point fingers to anyone, but examples of where you see these tweets, you know, what were they thinking?
Presumably there's an optimal strategy between the thought speech motor pathway, especially when the motor pathway engages communication with hundreds of thousands of people and retweets in particular and the cut and paste function and the screenshot function are often the reason why speech propagates.
So to me, it's a little eerie that just that the neural circuitry can do this and that we are catching up a little bit more slowly to the technology and you've got these casualties of that mismatch.
I think that's a good adjective to use, the casualties of what's going on. Because yes, it is the case with texting, what you're really losing there is less so the ability to write, but more the ability to interpret what is being written. And you can over-under-turp it something that somebody means.
On the flip side of that, you know, when if somebody's writing something very quick, they could be writing instinctually, more instinctually, their true meaning. And they don't have time to modify and color code what they're trying to say. And that's what they really feel, as opposed to saying in a more nuanced way.
So I think both sides of that casualty are present, and that's a downturn, an unintended negative consequence of short term, I mean short word communication.
Yeah, I agree that this whole phenomenon could be netting people that normally would only say these things out loud once inside the door of their own home, or not at all. Right. It's an interesting time that we're in vis-a-vis speech and language and motor patterns. It's a part of the human evolution for language. I think this is all part of our evolution. That's right.
So for those of you thinking terrible thoughts, please put them in the world and be a casualty. And for those of you that are not, please be very careful with how proficient your thought to language to motor action goes. Maybe the technology companies should install some buffers, some AI-based buffers.
Right, that's taking some EG signals from your brain while you're texting to say, okay, this is not a great thought, slow down. Right, this doesn't reflect your best state. That brings me to what was going to be the next question anyway, which is we are quickly moving toward a time where there will be an even faster transition from thought
to speech, to motor output, and maybe won't require motor output. What I'm referring to here is some of the incredible work of our colleagues Eddie Chang at UCSF and others who are taking paralyzed human beings and learning to translate the electrical signals of neurons in various areas, including speech and language areas, to computer screens that type out what these people are thinking. In other words, paralyzed people can put their thoughts on
into writing. That's a pretty extreme and wonderful example of recovery of function that is sure to continue to evolve. But I think we are headed toward a time not too long from now where my thoughts can be translated into words on a page if I allow that to happen.
Yeah, so, and Eddie Chang's work, which I admire quite a bit in sight in my papers, I think he's really one of those at the leading edge of trying to understand within humans the neurobiology of speech. And he may not say it directly, but I talked to him about this, it supports this idea that the speech circuit and the separate language module, I don't really think that there's a separation there.
So with that knowledge, yes, in putting electrodes in the human brain and then translating those electrical signals to speech currents, yeah, we can start to tell what is that person thinking, why? Because we often think in terms of speech and without saying words.
And that's a scary thought. And now imagine if you can now translate those into a signal that transmits something wirelessly and someone from some distant part of the planet is hearing your speech from a wireless signal without you speaking.
Probably that won't be done in an ethical way, who knows? The ethics of doing that probably might not happen, but who knows? We have these songbirds. We apply the same technique to them. We can start to hear what they're singing in their dreams or whatever, even though they don't produce sound. We can find out by testing on them. It's coming. One way or another, it's coming.
For those listening who are interested in getting better at speaking and understanding languages, are there any tools that you recommend? And here again, I realize you're not a speech therapist, but here I'm not thinking about ameliorating any kind of speech deficiency. I'm thinking, for instance, do you recommend that people read different types of writing? Would you recommend that people learn how to dance in order to become better at expressing themselves verbally?
And feel free to have some degrees of freedom in this answer. These are obviously not peer-reviewed studies that we're referring to, although there may be. But I'm struck by the number of things that you do exceedingly well, and I can't help but ask.
Well, the singing, which I realize it may, your brother didn't pay me to say this, may not be quite as good as your brother's yet, but is getting, you'll surpass him, I'm guessing, at some point, getting there. Exactly. There you go.
You know, should kids learn how to dance and read hard books and simple books? What do you recommend? Should adults learn how to do that? Everyone wants to know how to keep their brain working better, so to speak, but also I think people want to be able to speak well, and people want to be able to understand well.
Yeah, so what I've discovered personally, right, is that so when I switch from pursuing a career in science from a career in dance, I thought one day I would stop dancing, but I haven't because I find it fulfilling for me, you know, just as a life experience. So ever since I started college, you know, my
late teens in early 20s, I kept dancing even till this day.
And there have been periods of time like during the pandemic where I slowed down on dancing and so forth. And when you do that, you realize, OK, there are parts of your body where your muscle tone decreases a little bit and somewhat. Or you could start to gain weight. I somehow don't gain weight that easily. And I think it's related to my dance, if that's meaningful to your audience. But what I found is, in science, we like to think of a separation between movement and action and cognition.
And there is a separation between perception and production. Cognition being perception, production being movement, right? But if the speech pathways is next to the movement pathways, what I discover is by dancing, it is helping me think. It is helping keeping my brain fresh. It's not just moving my muscles. I'm using the circuitry in my brain to control a whole big body.
You need a lot of brain tissue to do that. And so I argue if you want to stay cognitively intact into your old age, you better be moving. And you better be doing it consistently, whether it's dancing, walking, running, and also practicing speech.
oratory speech and so forth or singing is controlling the brain circuits that are moving your facial musculature and it's going to keep your cognitive circuits also in tune. And I'm convinced of that from my own personal experience.
For me, long, slow runs are a wonderful way to kind of loosen the joints for long podcasts, especially the solo podcasts, which can take many hours to record. And without those long, slow runs, at least the day before or even the morning of, I don't think I could do it, at least not as well.
All right, well, you're experiencing something similar. So that's an NF2. Yeah, NF2. I'm tempted to learn how to dance because there are a lot of reasons to learn how to dance. People can use their imagination.
I definitely want to get the opportunity to talk about some of the newer work that you're into right now about genomes of animals. As you perhaps can tell from my quite authentic facial expressions, I adore the animal kingdom. I just find it amazing. And it's the reason I went into neurobiology in part. So many animals, so many different patterns of movement, so many body plans, so many specializations.
What is the value of learning the genomes of all these animals? You know, I can think of conservation based, you know, schemes of trying to preserve these precious critters. But what are you doing with the genomes of these animals? What do you want to understand about their brain circuits? And how does this relate to some of the discussion we've been having open to? Yeah, I've gotten very heavily involved in genomes. You know, not just to get an individual gene involved in the
trade of interest like spoken language. But I realized that nature has done natural experiments for us. But all these species out there with these various traits and the one that I'm studying like vocal learning has evolved multiple times among the animal kingdom, even if it's rare, it's multiple times.
And the similar genetic changes occurred in those species. But to find out what those genetic changes that are associated with the trait of interest and not some other trait like flying in birds, as opposed to singing, you have to do what's called comparative genomics, even in the context of studying the brain.
and you need their genomes to compare the genomes and do like a GWAS, a genome-wide association study, not just within a species like humans, but a cross-species. And so you need good genomes to do that. Plus, I've discovered I'm also interested in evolution in origins. How did these species come about a similar trait in the last 300 million years or 60 million years, depending on what you're talking about?
And you need a good phylogenetic tree to do that. And to get a good phylogenetic tree, you also need their genomes. And so because of this, I got involved in large-scale consortiums to produce genomes of many different species, including my vocal learners and their closest relatives that I'm fans of.
But I couldn't convince the funding agencies to give me the money to do that just for my own project. But when you get a whole bunch of people together who want to study various traits, you know, heart disease or loss and gain of flight and so forth, suddenly we all need lots of genomes to do this. And so now that got me into a project to lead something called a vertebrate genomes project to eventually sequence all 70,000 species on the planet.
and Earth bio-genome project, all eukaryotic species, all 2 million of them. And to no longer be in a situation where I wish I had this genome, now we have the genetic code of all life on the planet, create a database of all their traits, and find the genetic association with everything out there that makes a difference from one species to another.
One more piece of the equation to add to this story is what I didn't realize as a neuroscientist were that these genomes are not only incomplete, but there have lots of errors in them. False gene duplications where mother and father chromosomes were so different from each other that the genome algorithm, assembly algorithms treated them as two different genes in this part of the chromosome.
So there are a lot of these false duplicated genes that people were thought were real, but were not, or missing parts of the genome because the enzymes used to sequence the DNA couldn't get through this regulatory region that folded up on itself and made it hard to sequence.
And so I end up in these consortiums pulling in the genome sequencing companies developing the technology to work with us to improve it further. And the computer science guys who then take that data and that technology and try to make the complete genomes and make the algorithms better to produce
What we now just did recently, led by an effort by Adam Phillipi, is the first human telomere to telomere genome with no errors, all complete, no missing sequence. And now we're trying to do the same thing with vertebrates and other species. Actually, we improved that even before we got to what we call telomere to telomere from one end of chromosome to another.
And what we're discovering is in this dark matter of the genome that was missing before, turns out to be some regulatory regions that are specialized in vocal learning species, and we think are involved in developing speed circuits. Incredible.
Well, so much to learn and that we're going to learn from this information. Early on in these genome projects and connectome projects, I confess I was a little bit cynical. This would be about 10, 15 years ago. I thought, okay, necessary, but not sufficient for anything. We need it, but it's not clear what's going to happen, but you just gave a very clear example of what we stand to learn from this kind of information.
And I know from the conservation side, there's a huge interest in this because even though we would prefer to keep all these species alive rather than clone them, these sorts of projects do offer the possibility of potentially recreating species that were lost due to our own ignorance or missteps or what have you. Yes. And along those lines, because we got involved in genomics,
some of the first species that we start working on are critically endangered species. And I'm doing that not only for prospective to understand their brains and the genes involved in their brain function, but I feel like it's a moral duty. So the fact that now I've become more involved in genome biology and have helped develop these tools for more complete genomes, let's capture their genetic code now before they're gone.
And could we use that information to resurrect the species at some future time, if not in my lifetime, in the future in generations ahead of us? And so, in anticipation of that, we create a database we call the genome arc, and no pun intended, like Noah's Ark, meant to store the genetic code as complete genome assemblies as possible for all species on the planet.
to be used for basic science but also some point in the future. And because of that, funding agencies of private foundations that are interested in conservation have been reaching out to me now, a neuroscientist, to help them out in producing high quality genome data of endangered species that they can use like revive and restore who want to resurrect the passenger pigeon or colossal who wants to resurrect the woolly mammoth.
And so we're producing high quality genomes for these groups for the conservation projects. What a terrific and important initiative. And I think for those listening today, they now certainly understand the value of deeply understanding the brain structures and genomes of different species because
I confess, even though I knew a bit of the songbird literature, and I certainly understand that humans have speech and language, I had no idea that there was so much convergence of function structure and genomes. And to me, I feel a lot more like an ape than I do a songbird. And yet, here we are with the understanding
that there's a lot more similarity between songbirds and humans than I certainly ever thought before. Yeah, something very close to home for us humans, I can give you an example of, is evolution of skin color. And in skin color, we use it, unfortunately, for racism and so forth. We use it also for good things to let in more light or let out less light, depending on the part of the planet our population evolved in.
And most people think dark-skinned people all evolve from the same dark-skinned person and light-skinned people all evolve from the same light-skinned person, but that's not the case. Dark-skinned and light-skinned amongst humans has evolved independently multiple times, like in, you know, the Pacific Islands versus Africa.
And it's just depending on the angle of light hitting the earth as to whether you need more protection from the sun or less protection that's also associated with vitamin D synthesis in the skin.
And so, and each time where a darker or lighter skin evolved independently, it hit the same gene, you know, you know, the melanin, melanin receptors. That's right. Yes. Yeah. Genes that are involved in melanin formation.
And so those genes evolve some of the same mutations, even in different species. It's not just humans. In equatorial regions, they're darker skinned animals than going away from the equator. All right, I think of the arctic foxes. That's right. That's right. Polar bears, you know. And so some of the same genes are used in evolutionary perspective to evolve in a similar way within an across species. Incredible.
And that's the same thing happening in the brain, too. Language is no exception. Well, I have to say, as somebody who is a career neuroscientist, but as I mentioned several times now, who also adores the animal kingdom, but is also obsessed with speech and language and at a distance, not as a practitioner of
of music and dance. This has been an incredible conversation and opportunity for me to learn. I know I speak for a tremendous number of people, and I just really want to say thank you for joining us today. You are incredibly busy. It's clear from your description of your science and your knowledge base that you are involved in a huge number of things. Very busy, so thank you for taking the time to speak to all of us.
Thank you for the work that you're doing, both on speech and language, but also this important work on genomes and conservation of endangered species and far more. And I have to say, if you would agree to come back and speak to us again sometime, I'm certain that if we were to sit down even six months or a year from now, there's gonna be a lot more to come. Yeah, we have some things cooking. And thank you for inviting me here to get the word out to the community of what's going on in the science world.
Well, we're honored and very grateful to Eric. Thank you. Welcome. Thank you for joining me today for my discussion with Dr. Eric Jarvis. If you'd like to learn more about his laboratory's work, you can go to Jarvis Lab, spell J-A-R-V-I-S lab, all one word, JarvisLab.net. And there you can learn about all the various studies taking place in his laboratory, as well as some of the larger overarching themes that are driving those studies, including studies on human genomics and animal genomics.
that surely are going to lead to the next stage discoveries of how we learn and think about and indeed use language. If you're learning from and are enjoying this podcast, please subscribe to our YouTube channel. That's a simple zero cost way to support us. Please also subscribe to the podcast on Spotify and Apple. And on both Spotify and Apple, you have the opportunity to leave us up to a five star review.
If you have questions or comments or suggestions about topics you'd like us to cover or guests you'd like us to interview on the Huberman Lab podcast, please put those in the comments section on YouTube. We do read all those comments and we do take them to heart. Please also check out the sponsors mentioned at the beginning of today's podcast and check out momentous supplements, our new partners in the supplement space and check out Athletic Greens. That's the best way to support this podcast.
If you're not already following us on social media, please do so. We are Huberman Lab on Twitter and we are also Huberman Lab on Instagram and both places I cover science and science related tools, some of which overlap with the content of the Huberman Lab podcast, but much of which is unique from the content covered on the Huberman Lab podcast. Again, that's Huberman Lab on Instagram and Huberman Lab on Twitter.
Please also check out our neural network monthly newsletter. This is a newsletter that has summaries of podcast episodes. It also includes a lot of actionable protocols. It's very easy to sign up for the newsletter. You go to HubermanLab.com, click on the menu, go to newsletter. You supply your email, but we do not share your email with anybody. We have a very clear and rigorous privacy policy, which is we do not share your email with anybody. And the newsletter comes out once a month and it is completely zero cost. Again, just go to HubermanLab.com.
and go to the neural network newsletter. I'd also like to point out that the Huberman Lab podcast has a clips channel. So these are brief clips anywhere from three to 10 minutes that encompass single concepts and actionable protocols related to sleep, to focus interviews with various guests. We talk about things like caffeine, when to drink caffeine relative to sleep, alcohol, when and how and if anyone should ingest it relative to sleep.
dopamine, serotonin, mental health, physical health, and on and on, all the things that relate to the topics most of interest to you. You can find that easily by going to YouTube, look for Huberman Lab clips in the search area, and it will take you there, subscribe, and we are constantly updating those with new clips. This is especially useful, I believe, for people that have missed some of the earlier episodes, or you're still working through the back catalog of Huberman Lab podcasts, which admittedly can be rather long. And last, but certainly not least, thank you for your interest in science.
Was this transcript helpful?
Recent Episodes
Dr. David Anderson: The Biology of Aggression, Mating, & Arousal

Huberman Lab
Dr. David Anderson discusses human behavior, including violent aggression and fear, highlighting the role of hormones, context, and prior experience in shaping responses to threats and opportunities for mating; he also describes novel therapeutics for mental health.
September 12, 2022
Tools to Improve Your Focus & Concentration

Huberman Lab
Host shares a list of tools to improve focus, including transitioning into focus, maintaining focus during work, and exiting the focus session with support from science-backed strategies such as binaural beats and supplements like curcumin that enhance dopamine and epinephrine.
September 05, 2022
LIVE EVENT Q&A: Dr. Andrew Huberman Question & Answer in Portland, OR

Huberman Lab
The podcast host shares Q&A session answers from a live event in Portland on TBIs treatment, hyperbaric oxygen therapy, brain plasticity improvement, and more.
August 31, 2022
What Alcohol Does to Your Body, Brain & Health

Huberman Lab
Host discusses physiological effects of alcohol on brain and body over time, genetic differences that predispose certain individuals to alcoholism, and science-based strategies to mitigate hangover severity, while highlighting direct negative impacts of low-to-moderate consumption on mental and physical health.
August 22, 2022
Related Episodes
Dr. Eddie Chang: The Science of Learning & Speaking Languages

Huberman Lab
My guest is Eddie Chang, MD, a neurosurgeon and professor of neurological surgery at the University of California, San Francisco (UCSF) and the co-director of the Center for Neural Engineering & Prostheses. We discuss the brain mechanisms underlying speech, language learning and comprehension, communicating human emotion with words and hand gestures, bilingualism and language disorders, such as stuttering. Dr. Chang also explains his work developing and applying state-of-the-art technology to decode speech and using that information and artificial intelligence (AI) to successfully restore communication to patients who have suffered paralyzing injuries or “locked in syndrome.” We also discuss his work treating patients with epilepsy. Finally, we consider the future: how modern neuroscience is overturning textbook medical books, the impact of digital technology such as smartphones on language and the future of natural and computer-assisted human communication. For the full show notes, visit hubermanlab.com. Thank you to our sponsors AG1 (Athletic Greens): https://athleticgreens.com/huberman LMNT: https://drinklmnt.com/huberman Supplements from Momentous https://www.livemomentous.com/huberman Timestamps (00:00:00) Dr. Eddie Chang, Speech & Language (00:03:16) Sponsor: LMNT (00:07:19) Neuroplasticity, Learning of Speech & Environmental Sounds (00:13:10) White Noise Machines, Infant Sleep & Sensitization (00:17:26) Mapping Speech & Language in the Brain (00:24:26) Emotion; Anxiety & Epilepsy (00:30:19) Epilepsy, Medications & Neurosurgery (00:33:01) Ketogenic Diet & Epilepsy (00:34:04) Sponsor: AG1 (00:36:10) Absence Seizures, Nocturnal Seizures & Other Seizure Types (00:41:08) Brain Areas for Speech & Language, Broca’s & Wernicke’s Areas, New Findings (00:53:23) Lateralization of Speech/Language & Handedness, Strokes (00:59:05) Bilingualism, Shared Language Circuits (01:01:18) Speech vs. Language, Signal Transduction from Ear to Brain (01:12:38) Shaping Breath: Larynx, Vocal Folds & Pharynx; Vocalizations (01:17:37) Mapping Language in the Brain (01:20:26) Plosives & Consonant Clusters; Learning Multiple Languages (01:25:07) Motor Patterns of Speech & Language (01:28:33) Reading & Writing; Dyslexia & Treatments (01:34:47) Evolution of Language (01:37:54) Stroke & Foreign Accent Syndrome (01:40:31) Auditory Memory, Long-Term Motor Memory (01:45:26) Paralysis, ALS, “Locked-In Syndrome” & Brain Computer Interface (BCI) (02:02:14) Neuralink, BCI, Superhuman Skills & Augmentation (02:10:21) Non-Verbal Communication, Facial Expressions, BCI & Avatars (02:17:35) Stutter, Anxiety & Treatment (02:22:55) Tools: Practices for Maintaining Calm Under Extreme Demands (02:31:10) Zero-Cost Support, YouTube Feedback, Spotify & Apple Reviews, Sponsors, Momentous Supplements, Huberman Lab Premium, Neural Network Newsletter, Social Media Title Card Photo Credit: Mike Blabac Disclaimer
October 24, 2022
127 | Erich Jarvis on Language, Birds, and People

Sean Carroll's Mindscape: Science, Society, Philosophy, Culture, Arts, and Ideas
Erich Jarvis, neurogenetics of language researcher, discusses his findings on vocal learning in parrots and comparisons with humans, suggesting similar mental circuits responsible for language development and implications for intelligence.
December 14, 2020
Is Music the Language of the Universe? with Daniel Levitin

StarTalk Radio
Neil deGrasse Tyson and Chuck Nice explore how music aids memory recall, discuss the Singing Neanderthal Theory, delve into brain networks like the default mode network, and examine the therapeutic benefits of music with neuroscientist and author Daniel Levitin.
October 04, 2024
Abel James: How to Upgrade Your Brain with Music, Beat Stress & Rock Your Life

The Fat-Burning Man Show with Abel James: Real Food, Real Results
Podcast covers Alyson interviewing about brain upgrades, music's connection with testosterone, achieving full-brain workouts, and various other topics.
November 13, 2015

Ask this episodeAI Anything

Hi! You're chatting with Huberman Lab AI.
I can answer your questions from this episode and play episode clips relevant to your question.
You can ask a direct question or get started with below questions -
What is the primary focus of Dr. Eric Jarvis's research?
How do speech and language interact in the brain?
What genes aid in muscle firing during speech?
How does cultural evolution relate to language evolution?
What role do motor movements play in communication?
Sign In to save message history